Reading aloud polysyllabic words

Part of the Neuropsychology and Cognition book series (NPCO, volume 22)

Abstract

Although a great deal is known about the naming process of monosyllabic words (see for instance Coltheart, Curtis, Atkins, & Haller, 1993; Norris, 1994; Plaut, McClelland, Seidenberg, & Patterson, 1996; Seidenberg & McClelland, 1989; Zorzi, Houghton, & Butterworth, 1998), very few studies have been devoted to the naming process of polysyllabic words (see however, Ans, Carbonnel, & Valdois, 1998; Jared & Seidenberg, 1990). Indeed, most of the studies on word naming concern monosyllabic words. This is a paradox since monosyllabic words represent less than 10 % of the lexicon and therefore can be considered as the exception, whereas polysyllabic words should be studied more extensively. The present chapter addresses explicitly the processes involved in generating the pronunciations of polysyllabic words from their written forms. In particular, the focus of the present chapter is not lexical access per se but is the process of producing a phonological code for polysyllabic words. In this chapter, we will argue that the syllable is a likely processing unit of reading aloud polysyllabic words. We will present empirical evidence and then we will discuss different models of polysyllabic words.

Keywords

Lexical Decision Task Visual Word Recognition Naming Latency Word Naming Monosyllabic Word 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Ans, B., Carbonnel, S., & Valdois, S. (1998). A connectionist multiple–trace memory model for polysyllabic word reading. Psychological Review, 105, 678–723.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bachoud–Lévi, A–C, Dupoux, E., Cohen, L., & Mehler, J. (1998). Where is the length effect? A cross–linguistic study of speech production. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 331–346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bowey, J. A. (1990). Orthographic onsets and rimes as functional units of reading. Memory & Cognition,18, 419–427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Carreiras, M., Alvarez, C.J., & De Vega, M. (1993). Syllable frequency and visual word recognition in Spanish. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 766–780.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Coltheart, M. (1978). Lexical access in simple reading tasks. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Strategies ofinformation processing (pp. 151–216). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  6. Coltheart, M., Curtis, B., Atkins, P., & Haller, M. (1993). Models of reading aloud: Dual–route and parallel–processing–distributed approaches. Psychological Review, 100, 589–608.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Cutler, A. (1997). The syllable’s role in the segmentation of stress languages. Language and CognitiveProcesses, 12, 839–845.Google Scholar
  8. Cutler, A., Mehler, J., Norris, D., & Segui, J. (1986). The syllable’s differing role in the segmentation of French and English. Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 385–400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Eriksen, C.W., Pollack, M.D., & Montague, W.E. (1970). Implicit speech: Mechanism in perceptual coding? Journal of Experimental Psychology, 84, 502–507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Ferrand, L. (in press). Reading aloud polysyllabic words and non–words: The syllabic–length effect reexamined. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.Google Scholar
  11. Ferrand, L., & Grainger, J. (1992). Phonology and orthography in visual word recognition: Evidence from masked non-word priming. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 42A, 353–372.Google Scholar
  12. Ferrand, L., & Grainger, J. (1993). The time–course of orthographic and phonological code activation in the early phases of visual word recognition. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 31, 119–122.Google Scholar
  13. Ferrand, L., & Grainger, J. (1994). Effects of orthography are independent of phonology in masked form priming. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47A, 365–382.Google Scholar
  14. Ferrand, L., & Grainger, J. (1996). List context effects on masked phonological priming in the lexical decision task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3, 515–519.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Ferrand, L., & Segui, J. (1998). The syllable’s role in speech production: Are syllables chunks, schémas, or both? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, 253–258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Ferrand, L., Grainger, J., & Segui, J. (1994). A study of masked form priming in picture naming and word naming. Memory & Cognition, 22, 431–441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Ferrand, L., Segui, J., & Grainger, J. (1996). Masked priming of word and picture naming: The role of syllabic units. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 708–723.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Ferrand, L., Segui, J., & Humphreys, G.W. (1997). The syllable’s role in word naming. Memory &Cognition, 25,458–470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Forster, K.I., & Chambers, S. (1973). Lexical access and naming time. Journal of Verbal Learning andVerbal Behavior, 12, 627–635.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Forster K.I., & Davis, C. (1984). Repetition priming and frequency attenuation in lexical access. Journalof Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 680–698.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Forster, K.I., & Davis, C. (1991). The density constraint on form–priming in the naming task: Interference effects from a masked prime. Journal of Memory and Language, 30,1–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Frederiksen, J.R., & Kroll, J.F. (1976). Spelling and sound: Approaches to the internal lexicon. Journal ofExperimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2, 361–379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Grainger, J. (1990). Word frequency and neighborhood frequency effects in lexical decision and naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 228–244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Grainger, J., & Ferrand, L. (1996). Masked orthographic and phonological priming in visual word recognition and naming: Cross–task comparisons. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 623–647.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Grainger, J., & Jacobs, A.M. (1996). Orthographic processing in visual word recognition: A multiple read–out model. Psychological Review, 103, 518–565.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Henderson, L. (1982). Orthography and word recognition in reading. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  27. Jacobs, A.M., Grainger, J., & Ferrand, L. (1995). The incremental priming technique: A method for determining within–condition priming effects. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 1101–1110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Jacobs, A.M., Rey, A., Ziegler, J., & Grainger, J. (1998). MROM–P: An interactive activation, multiple read–out model of orthographic and phonological processes in visual word recognition. In J. Grainger & A.M. Jacobs (Eds.), Localist connectionist approaches to human cognition. Hillsdale, NJ; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  29. Jared, D. (1997). Evidence that strategy effects in word naming reflect changes in output timing rather than changes in processing route. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, andCognition, 23, 1424–1438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Jared, D., & Seidenberg, M.S. (1990). Naming multi –syllabic words. Journal of ExperimentalPsychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 92–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Jared, D., McRae, K., & Seidenberg, M.S. (1990). The basis of consistency effects in word naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 687–715.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Klapp, S.T., & Wyatt, E.T. (1976). Motor programming within a sequence of responses. Journal of MotorBehavior, 8, 19–26.Google Scholar
  33. Klapp, S.T., & Erwin, C.I. (1976). Relation between programming time and duration of the response being programmed. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2, 591–598.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Klapp, S.T., Anderson, W.G., & Berrian, R.W. (1973). Implicit speech in reading reconsidered. Journalof Experimental Psychology, 100, 368–374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Loftus, G.R. (1993). A picture is worth a thousand p values: On the irrelevance of hypothesis testing in the microcomputer age. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 25, 250–256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Lupker, S.J., Brown, P., & Colombo, L (1997). Strategic control in a naming task: Changing routes or changing deadlines? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 570–590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Mason, M. (1978). From print to sound in mature readers as a function of reader ability and two forms of orthographic regularity. Memory & Cognition, 6, 568–581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Mewhort, D.J.K., & Beal, A.L. (1977). Mechanisms of word identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 3, 629–640.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Millis, M.L. (1986). Syllables and spelling units affect feature integration in words. Memory andCognition, 14, 409–419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Norris, D. (1994). A quantitative, multiple levels model of reading aloud. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 1212–1232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Perfetti, CA., & Bell, L.C. (1991). Phonemic activation during the first 40 ms of word identification: Evidence from backward masking and priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 30,473–485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Plaut, D.C., McClelland, J.L., Seidenberg, M.S., & Patterson, K. (1996). Understanding normal and impaired word reading: Computational principles in quasi–regular domains. Psychological Review,103, 56–115.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Prinzmetal, W., Hoffman, H., & Vest, K. (1991). Automatic processes in word perception: An analysis from illusory conjonctions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception andPerformance, 17, 902–923.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Prinzmetal, W., Treiman, R., & Rho, S.H. (1986). How to see a reading unit. Journal of Memory andLanguage, 25, 461–475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Rapp, B.C. (1992). The nature of sub–lexical orthographic organization: The bigram trough hypothesis examined. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 33–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Rastle, K., & Coltheart, M. (1998). Whammies and double whammies: The effect of length on non–word reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, 277–282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Richardson, J.T.E. (1976). The effect of stimulus attributes upon latency of word recognition. BritishJournal of Psychology, 67, 315–325.Google Scholar
  48. Schiller, N.O. (1998). The effect of visually masked syllable primes on the naming latencies of words and pictures. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 484–507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Segui, J., Dupoux, E., & Mehler, J. (1990). The role of the syllable in speech segmentation, phoneme identification, and lexical access. In G.T.M. Altmann (Ed.), Cognitive models of speech processing: Psycholinguistic and computational perspectives. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  50. Seidenberg, M.S. (1987). Sub–lexical structures in visual word recognition: Access units or orthographic redundancy? In M. Coltheart (Ed.), The psychology of reading, Vol. XII of Attention and Performance (pp. 245–263). Hillsdale: LEAGoogle Scholar
  51. Seidenberg, M.S. (1989). Reading complex words. In G. Carlson & M. Tanenhaus (Eds.), Linguisticstructure in language processing(pp. 53–105). Boston: Kluwer Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
  52. Seidenberg, M.S., & McClelland, J.L. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of visual word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96, 523–568.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Spoehr, K.T. (1978). Phonological encoding in visual word recognition. Journal of Verbal Learning andVerbal Behavior, 17, 127–142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Spoehr, K.T. (1981). Word recognition in speech and reading: Toward a single theory of language processing. In P. D. Eimas & J.L. Miller (Eds.), Perspectives in the study of speech (pp. 239–282). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  55. Spoehr, K.T., & Smith, E.E. (1973). The role of the syllables in perceptual processing. CognitivePsychology, 5,71–89.Google Scholar
  56. Stone, G.O., & Van Orden, G.C. (1993). Strategic control of processing in word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19,144–774.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Taft, M. (1979). Lexical access via an orthographic code: The Basic Orthographic Syllabic Structure (BOSS). Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 21–39CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Tousman, S., & Inhoff, A. (1992). Phonology in multi–syllabic word recognition. Journal ofPsycholinguistic Research, 21, 525–544.Google Scholar
  59. Treiman, R. (1986). The division between onsets and rimes in English syllables. Journal of Memory andLanguage, 25, 476–491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Treiman, R. (1990). To what extent do orthographic units in print mirror phonological units in speech? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research , 23, 91 –110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Treiman, R., & Chafetz, J. (1987). Are there onset– and rime–like units in written words? In M. Coltheart (Ed.), Attention and Performance XII: The Psychology of reading(pp. 281 –298). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  62. Treiman, R., & Kessler, B. (1995). In defense of an onset–rime syllable structure of English. Languageand Speech, 38, 127–142.Google Scholar
  63. Treiman, R., & Zukowski, A. (1988). Units in reading and spelling. Journal of Memory and Language,27, 466–477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Weekes, B.S. (1997). Differential effects of number of letters on word and non–word naming latency. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50A, 439–456.Google Scholar
  65. Ziegler, J.C., Ferrand, L., Jacobs, A.M., Rey, A., & Grainger, J. (in press). Visual and phonological codes in letter and word recognition: Evidence from incremental priming. Quarterly Journal ofExperimental Psychology.Google Scholar
  66. Zorzi, M., Houghton, G., & Butterworth, B. (1998). Two routes or one in reading aloud? A connectionist dual–process model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1131–1161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Zwitserlood, P., Schriefers, H., Lahiri, A., & van Donselaar, W. (1993). The role of syllables in the perception of spoken Dutch. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition,19, 260–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2003

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Laboratoire de Psychologie ExpérimentaleCNRS and Université René DescartesBoulogne-BillancourtFrance

Personalised recommendations