The Motor Theory of Voluntary Thinking

  • Barry H. Cohen

Abstract

The theory proposed in this chapter—the motor theory of voluntary thinking (MTVT)—is not exactly new; its basic elements can be found in the motor theories of Bain (1888), Maudsley (1889), Ribot (1889), Pillsbury (1908), and Washburn (1916) to name just a few. Many of the ideas expressed below were commonplace at the beginning of this century, and the interested reader is encouraged to explore the original sources referenced within this chapter, which frequently contain detailed, anecdotal descriptions and lengthy logical arguments in support of these ideas. To maintain the clarity of my exposition, I will quote only a few examples from the early literature; it is not my intention to present a history of the theoretical formulations concerning the relation between the motor system and attention or thought (a brief history can be found in Smith, 1969). Rather, my purpose in writing the present chapter is to present some of these old ideas in the context of a cohesive theory that is as clear, plausible, and useful as possible.

Keywords

Motor Activity Visual Image Motor Pattern Motor Theory Visual Imagery 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Antrobus, J. S., Antrobus, T. S., and Singer, J. L. (1964). Eye movements accompanying daydreaming, visual imagery and thought suppression. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 69, 244–252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bain, A. (1888). The emotions and the will. London: Parker.Google Scholar
  3. Bartoshuk, A. K. (1956). EMG gradients and EEG amplitude during motivated listening. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 10, 156–164.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Basmajian, J. V. (1979). Muscles alive: Their functions revealed by electromyography. Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins.Google Scholar
  5. Bernhaut, M., Gellhorn, E., and Rasmussen, A. T. (1953). Experimental contributions to the problem of consciousness. Journal of Neurophysiology, 16, 21–35.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Bloomfield, H. H., Cain, M. P., and Jaffe, D. T. (1975). Transcendental meditation: Discovering inner energy and overcoming stress. New York: Delacorte.Google Scholar
  7. Brown, B. B. (1968). Visual recall ability and eye movements. Psychophysiology, 4, 300–306.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Cacioppo, J. T., and Petty, R. E. (1981). Electromyographic specificity during covert information processing. Psychophysiology, 18, 518–523.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Cohen, B. H. ( 1978, September). Role of the motor system in visual imagery. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, Toronto, Canada.Google Scholar
  10. Cohen, B. H. (1983). Electromyographic patterns associated with cognition and affect. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, New York University.Google Scholar
  11. Cohen, B. H., and Jonas, C. (1978, November). A closer look at the visualizer: Habit or vividness? Paper presented at the second annual convention on the Imagery and Fantasy Process, Chicago.Google Scholar
  12. Cole, R. A., and Young, M. (1975). Effect of subvocalization on memory for speech sounds. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 1, 772–779.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Cuthbert, B. N., and Lang, P. J. (1984). Eye movements in visual imagery. Physiology, 21, 574.Google Scholar
  14. Deckert, G. H. (1964). Pursuit eye movements in the absence of a moving visual stimulus. Science, 143, 1192–1193.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Dunlap, K. (1927). The short-circuiting of conscious responses. Journal of Philosophy, 24, 263–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Ehrlichman, H., and Barrett, J. (1983). ‘Random’ saccadic eye movements during verbal-linguistic and visual-imaginal tasks. Acta Psychologica, 53, 9–26.Google Scholar
  17. Ekman, P., Friesen, W., and Ellsworth, P. (1972). Emotion in the human face. New York: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
  18. Evarts, E. V. (1971). Feedback and corollary discharge: A merging of the concepts. Neurosciences Research Program Bulletin, 9, 86–112.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Fridlund, A. J., and Izard, C. E. (1984). Electromyographic studies of facial expressions of emotions and patterns of emotions. In J. T. CACIOPPO and R. E. PETTY (Eds.), Social psychophysiology: A sourcebook, New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  20. Geisser, S., and Greenhouse, S. W. (1958). An extension of Box’s results on the use of the F distribution in multivariate analysis. Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 29, 885–891.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Gellhorn, E. (1958). The influence of curare on hypothalamic excitability and the elec- troencephalogram. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 10, 697–703.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hardyck, C. D., and Petrovich, L. F. (1970). Subvocal speech and comprehension level as a function of the difficulty of reading material. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9, 647–652.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Hebb, D. O. (1968). Concerning imagery. Psychological Review, 75, 466–477.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hefferline, R. F., and Perera, T. B. (1963). Proprioceptive discrimination of a covert operant without its observation by the subject. Science, 139, 834–835.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Jacobson, E. (1930). Electrical measurements of neuromuscular states during mental activities. III. Visual imagination and recollection. American Journal of Physiology, 95, 694702.Google Scholar
  26. Jacobson, E. (1932). Electrophysiology of mental activities. American Journal of Psychology, 44, 677–694.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Jacobson, E. (1938). Progressive relaxation (2nd ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. JACOBSON, E. (1964). Anxiety and tension control: A physiologic approach. Philadelphia: Lippincott.Google Scholar
  28. James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Holt. ( Reprinted: New York: Dover, 1950 ).Google Scholar
  29. Lacey, J. I. (1967). Somatic response patterning and stress: Some revisions of activation theory. In M. H. APPLEY and R. TRUMBULL (Eds.), Psychological stress: Issues in research. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  30. Lenox, J. R., Lange, A. F., and Graham, K. R. (1970). Eye movement amplitude in imagined pursuit of a pendulum with the eyes closed. Psychophysiology, 6, 773–777.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Leuba, C., Birch, L., and Appleton, J. (1968). Human problem solving during complete paralysis of the voluntary musculature. Psychological Reports, 22, 849–855.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Libby, W. L., JR., Lacey, B. C., and Lacey, J. I. (1973). Pupillary and cardiac activity during visual attention. Psychophysiology, 10, 270–294.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Luria, A. R. (1966). Higher cortical functions in man. London: Tavistock.Google Scholar
  34. Marks, D. F. (1973). Visual imagery differences in eye movements in recall of pictures. Perception and Psychophysics, 14, 407–412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Matin, L., Pearce, D., Matin, E., and Kibler, G. (1966). Visual perception of direction in the dark: Roles of local signs, eye movements and ocular proprioception. Vision Research, 6, 453–469.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Maudsley, H. (1889). The physiology of mind. New York: D. Appleton.Google Scholar
  37. McGuigan, F. J. (1978). Cognitive psychophysiology: Principles of covert behavior. New Jersey: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  38. McGuigan, F. J., and Rodier, W. I., III. (1968). Effects of auditory stimulation on covert oral behavior during silent reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 76, 649–655.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. McGuigan, F. J., and Winstead, JR., C. L. (1974). Discriminative relationship between covert oral behavior and the phonemic system in internal information processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 103, 885–890.Google Scholar
  40. Naranjo, C., and Ornstein, R. E. (1971). On the psychology of meditation. New York: Viking.Google Scholar
  41. Norman, D. A., and Shallice, T. (1985). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. In R. J. Davidson, G. E. Schwartz, and D. SHAPIRO (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation: Advances in research and theory, Vol. 4. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  42. Obrist, P. A., Webb, R. A., and Sutterer, J. R. (1969). Heart rate and somatic changes during aversive conditioning and a simple reaction-time task. Psychophysiology, 5, 696–723.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Obrist, P.A., Webb, R. A., Sutterer, J.R., and Howard, J. L. (1970). The cardiac-somatic relationship: Some reformulations. Psychophysiology, 6, 569–587.Google Scholar
  44. Pillsbury, W. B. (1908). Attention. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  45. Pinneo, L. R. (1961). The effects of induced muscle tension during tracking on level of activation and on performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 523–531.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Pope, K. S., and Singer, J. L. (1978). Regulation of the stream of consciousness: Toward a theory of ongoing thought. In G. E. SCHWARTZ and D. SHAPIRO (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation: Advances in research and theory. Vol. 2. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  47. Ribot, T. A. (1889). The psychology of attention. New York: Humboldt.Google Scholar
  48. Shaw, W. A. (1938). The distribution of muscular action potentials during imaging. Psychological Record, 2, 195–216.Google Scholar
  49. Shiffrin, R. M., and Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84, 127–190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Singer, J. L. (1975). The inner world of daydreaming. New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
  51. Singer, J. L., and Antrobus, J. S. (1965). Eye movement during fantasies. Archives of General Psychiatry, 12, 71–76.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Smith, M. O. (1969). History of the motor theories of attention. Journal of General Psychology, 80, 243–257.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Smith, S. M., Brown, H. 0., Toman, J. E. P., and Goodman, L. S. (1947). The lack of cerebral effects of d-tubocurarine. Anesthesiology, 8, 1–14.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Sperry, R. W. (1952). Neurology and the mind-brain problem. American Scientist, 40, 29 1312.Google Scholar
  55. Stoy, E. G. (1930). A preliminary study of ocular attitudes in thinking of spatial relations. Journal of General Psychology, 4, 379–385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Totten, E. (1935). Eye movement during visual imagery. Comparative Psychology Monographs, 11, No. 3.Google Scholar
  57. Wallerstein, H. (1954). An electromyographic study of attentive listening. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 8, 228–238.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Washburn, M. F. (1916). Movement and mental imagery. New York: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
  59. Weimer, W. B. (1977). A conceptual framework for cognitive psychology: Motor theories of the mind. In R. SHAW and J. BRANSFORD (Eds.), Perceiving, acting, and knowing. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1986

Authors and Affiliations

  • Barry H. Cohen
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyNew York UniversityUSA

Personalised recommendations