Skip to main content

Re-covering the Idea of a Tertiary Artifact

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Cultural-Historical Approaches to Studying Learning and Development

Part of the book series: Perspectives in Cultural-Historical Research ((PCHR,volume 6))

Abstract

Drawing on Wartofsky’s work on models and, in particular, his notion of a tertiary artifact as a guide to action, Cole gives detailed examples of how his creation of the Fifth Dimension, aiming at engaging vulnerable learners, can be characterized as a tertiary artifact that opens up imaginative possibilities for these learners. The argument is that there no natural perception; children must learn how to see, and how to think, and the construction of material representations, such as the environment offered by the Fifth Dimension, is key to this learning. A tertiary artifact mediates the ways in which we perceive the world in a particularly powerful way, guiding and informing human imagination. What tertiary artifacts permit and foster is a mode of engagement in the world that can be said to be offline. Offline activity, according to Wartofsky, is play in an imaginatively constructed world and this is what is offered in the design of the Fifth Dimension (5th D).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 139.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Our presentations were published several years later in an edited selection of papers from the conference (Engestrom, Meittinen, & Punamaki, 1990).

  2. 2.

    Similar examples can be found in Susi (2006) who suggests adding ‘where-from’ artifacts to Engestrom’s list see also McDonald, Huang Le, Higgins and Podmore (2005).

  3. 3.

    For video’s about various instantiations of the 5th D see lchcautbio.ucsd.edu.

  4. 4.

    For descriptions of other sites see Cole et al. (2006); uclinks.berkeley.edu.

References

  • Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology: A once and future discipline. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M. (1999). Cultural psychology: Some general principles and a concrete example. In Y. Engeström & R. L. Punamaki (Eds.), Perspective on activity theory (pp. 87–106). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M. (2006). Culture and cognitive development in phylogenetic, historical development in phylogenetic, historical, and ontogenetic perspective. In D. Kuhn & R. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of Child Psychology (Vol. 2): Cognition, perception and language (6th ed.). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M., & Packer, M. (2016a). Design-based intervention research as the science of the doubly artificial. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 25(4), 503–530.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M. & Packer, M. (2016b). A bio-cultural-historical approach to the study of development. In M. J. Gelfand, C. Chiu, & Huang & Y. Huang (Eds.), Handbook of advances in culture and psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 1–76). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dahl, B. (2002). Profit, learning, and ideology: A comparative study of institutional structure and idioculture in two after-school child enrichment programs. Honors Thesis, University of California, San Diego.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engeström, Y. (1990). Learning, working. Orienta Konsultit Oy: Imagining. Helsinki.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engeström, Y. (2007). Putting Vygotsky to work: The change laboratory as an application of double stimulation. In H. Daniels, M. Cole, & J. V. Wertsch (Eds.), The Cambridge companion to Vygotsky (pp. 363–382). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engeström, Y., Meittinen, R., & Punamaki, R.-L. (Eds.). (1999). Perspectves on activity theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gillespie, A., & Zittoun, T. (2010). Using resources: conceptualizing the mediation and reflective use of tools and signs. Culture and Psychology, 16(1), 37–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gillespie, A., & Zittoun, T. (2016). Imagination in human and cultural development. Hove: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Habib, L., & Wittek, L. (2007). The portfolio as artifact and actor. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 14(4), 266–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hedegaard, M. (1999). Activity theory and history teaching. In Y. Engeström, R. Meittinen, & R.-L. Punamaki (Eds.), Perspectves on activity theory (pp. 282–297). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hedegaard, M. (2005). Learning and child development. Aarhus: Aarhus University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirsh, A. (2015). IDPs at work. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 59(1), 77–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. (2014). The cultural ecosystem of human cognition. Philosophical Psychology, 7(1), 34–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • LCHC (The Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition) (1983). Culture and cognitive development. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), W. Kessen (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 295–356). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDonald, G., Le, H., Higgins, J., & Podmore, V. (2005). Artifacts, tools, and classrooms. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 12(2), 113–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miettinen, R. (1999). The riddle of things: Activity theory and actor-network theory as approaches to studying innovations. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 6(4), 170–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Susi, T. (2006). Tools and artefacts—knowing ‘where-from’ affects their present use. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 28(28).

    Google Scholar 

  • Wartofsky, M. (1979). Models: Representation and the scientific understanding. Dordrecht: Reidel.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. (1999). Dialogic inquiry. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Zittoun, T., & Glavenau, V. (2017). Handbook of imagination and culture. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mike Cole .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2019 Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd.

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Cole, M. (2019). Re-covering the Idea of a Tertiary Artifact. In: Edwards, A., Fleer, M., Bøttcher, L. (eds) Cultural-Historical Approaches to Studying Learning and Development. Perspectives in Cultural-Historical Research, vol 6. Springer, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-13-6826-4_20

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-13-6826-4_20

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Singapore

  • Print ISBN: 978-981-13-6825-7

  • Online ISBN: 978-981-13-6826-4

  • eBook Packages: EducationEducation (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics