Skip to main content

Connecting Animal Cognition and Emotion with Ethical Reasoning in the Classroom

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Animals in Environmental Education

Abstract

We developed an interdisciplinary course connecting the science of animal cognition and emotion with ethical reasoning for the general education curriculum at Roanoke College. The course engages students in considering familiar animals, their pets, first, followed by a broader phylogenetic range of species. Students learn about cultural views of animals and scientific evidence of high-level animal cognition and emotion. We ask students to analyze ways in which these culturally based views and scientific findings should impact our ethical reasoning about animals. In our surveys of students (n = 89) in six sections of the course, taught over three years, students reported that they valued the interdisciplinary approach of the course and that their knowledge of cognitive abilities of animals increased.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    We use the term “animal” rather than “non-human animal” throughout the chapter as a means of signifying non-human animal or creature that is “other than human” for shorthand convenience. We recognize that applying the term animal to creatures that are “other than human” privileges the human while failing to acknowledge that humans are animals.

  2. 2.

    Paul Shepard introduced the “Pleistocene Paradigm” to deep ecology, positing that humans depend on contact with nature and animals in order to develop past an adolescent state and therefore many modern humans in post-agricultural society are trapped in infantilism. He was a Professor of V.S. Banschbach’s at the Claremont Colleges.

  3. 3.

    And Bekoff (2007) prompts us to consider other reasons why the scientific study of animal emotions has progressed so slowly: namely the threat to use of animals in research, particularly animals kept in laboratories, but in our practice in ethological study, we learn what the methodological limitations to progress are.

  4. 4.

    Roanoke College IRB Approval #16115.

References

  • Banschbach, V. S., Brunelle, A., Bartlett, K., Grivetti, J., & Yeamans, R. L. (2006). Tool use by the forest ant, Aphaenogaster rudis: Ecology and task allocation. Insectes Sociaux, 53, 463–471.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bastian, B. (2011, March 23). The meat paradox: How we can love some animals and eat others. The Conversation. Retrieved from http://theconversation.com/the-meat-paradox-how-we-can-love-some-animals-and-eat-others-149

  • Bateson, M., Desire, S., Gartside, S. E., & Wright, G. A. (2011). Agitated honeybees exhibit pessimistic cognitive biases. Current Biology, 21(12), 1070–1073.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bekoff, M. (2007). The emotional lives of animals. Novato, CA: New World Library.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, C. (2008). The zoological connection: Animal-related human behavior. In C. P. Flynn (Ed.), Social creatures: A human and animal studies reader (pp. 7–21). New York: Lantern Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chawla, L. (2009). Growing up green: Becoming an agent of care for the natural world. Journal of Developmental Processes, 4(1), 6–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chawla, L., & Cushing, D. F. (2007). Education for strategic environmental behavior. Environmental Education Research, 13(4), 437–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Darst, R. G, & Dawson, J. I. (2019). Putting meat on the (classroom) table: Problems of denial and communication. In T. Lloro-Bidart & V. S. Banschbach (Eds.), Animals in environmental education: Interdisciplinary approaches to curriculum and pedagogy (p. tbd). London: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Derrida, J., & Wills, D. (2002). The animal that therefore I am (more to follow). Critical Inquiry, 28(2), 369–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dobnik, V. (2017, March 16). New York court asked to determine if chimp is legally a person. Chicago Tribune. Retrieved from http://www.chicagotribune.com/news/nationworld/ct-chimpanzee-rights-20170316-story.html

  • Goodall, J. (1968). The behaviour of free-living chimpanzees in the Gombe Stream Reserve. Animal Behaviour Monographs, 1, 161–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goralnik, L., & Nelson, M. P. (2015). Empathy and agency in the Isle Royale field philosophy experience. Journal of Sustainability Education, 10. Retrieved from http://www.jsedimensions.org/wordpress/wp-content/uploads/2015/12/Goralnik-Nelson-JSE-Nov-2015-Hope-Issue-PDF.pdf

  • Haraway, D. (1991). Simians, cyborgs and women: The reinvention of partial perspective. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kollmuss, A., & Agyeman, J. (2010). Mind the gap: Why do people act environmentally and what are the barriers to pro-environmental behavior? Environmental Education Research, 8(3), 239–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lloro-Bidart, T. (2015). A political ecology of education in/for the Anthropocene. Environment and Society: Advances in Research, 6, 128–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, M. R., & Russell, J. (2016). How could it be? Calling for science curricula that cultivate morals and values towards other animals and nature. Cultural Studies of Science Education, 11(4), 1023–1028.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Low, T. (2002). The new nature: Winners and losers in wild Australia. Melbourne, VIC: Penguin Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mill, J. S. (1998). Utilitarianism. R. Crisp (Ed.). London: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morell, V. (2013). Animal wise: The thoughts and emotions of our fellow creatures. New York: Crown Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oakley, J. (2012). Animality and environmental education: Toward an interspecies paradigm. Canadian Journal of Environmental Education, 16, 8–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pacini-Ketchabaw, V., & Nxumalo, F. (2015). Unruly raccoons and troubled educators: Nature/culture divides in a childcare centre. Environmental Humanities, 7, 151–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roanoke College. (2015). Intellectual inquiry: The core liberal arts curriculum at Roanoke College. Retrieved from https://www.roanoke.edu/Documents/AcademicAffairs/Gen%20Ed/INQ%20description%202015.docx

  • Russell, C. (1999). Problematizing nature experience in environmental education. The interrelationship of experience and story. The Journal of Experimental Education, 22(3), 123–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, C. (2005). Whoever does not write is written: The role of ‘nature’ in post-post approaches to environmental education research. Environmental Education Research, 11(4), 433–443.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, C., & Semenko, K. (2016). We take “cow” as a compliment: Fattening humane, environmental, and social justice education. In E. Cameron & C. Russell (Eds.), The fat pedagogy reader: Challenging weight-based oppression through critical pedagogy (pp. 211–220). New York: Peter Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sax, B. (2001). The mythical zoo: An A to Z of animals in world myth, legend and literature. Santa Barbara, CA: ABC-Clio, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepard, P. (1998). Thinking animals: Animals and the development of human intelligence. Athens, GA: University of Georgia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spannring, R. (2017). Animals in environmental education research. Environmental Education Research, 23(1), 63–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, R. B. (2007). Schooling and environmental education: Contradictions in purpose and practice. Environmental Education Research, 13(2), 139–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, D. R. (2006). A general inductive approach for analyzing qualitative evaluation data. American Journal of Evaluation, 27(2), 237–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen, N. (1963). On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 20, 410–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Traer, R. (2013). Doing environmental ethics. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Valerie S. Banschbach .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2019 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Banschbach, V.S., Larson-Harris, M. (2019). Connecting Animal Cognition and Emotion with Ethical Reasoning in the Classroom. In: Lloro-Bidart, T., Banschbach, V.S. (eds) Animals in Environmental Education. Palgrave Studies in Education and the Environment. Palgrave Macmillan, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-98479-7_11

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-98479-7_11

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Palgrave Macmillan, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-98478-0

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-98479-7

  • eBook Packages: EducationEducation (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics