Skip to main content

Dream Behavior and Dream Content in Healthy Persons

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Researching Dreams

Abstract

Dream-related behaviors like sharing dreams are best measured via questionnaires whereas dream content analysis provides information about dream content, for example, bizarreness of dreams, emotional tone of dreams. The main focus of this chapter is the so-called continuity hypothesis of dreaming, stating that dreams reflect the waking-life experiences of the dreamer. Research had identified factors that affect this continuity between waking and dreaming, for example, emotional intensity of the waking-life experience or the type of the waking-life activity (studying vs. meeting with friends). Also, gender differences in dream content seem to be continuous to gender differences found in waking life.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 69.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 99.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Agargün, M. Y., Kara, H., Bilici, M., Cilli, A. S., Telci, M., Semiz, Ü. B., & Basoglu, C. (1999). The Van Dream Anxiety Scale: A subjective measure of dream anxiety in nightmare sufferers. Sleep and Hypnosis, 1, 204–211.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arnulf, I., Grosliere, L., Le Corvec, T., Golmard, J.-L., Lascols, O., & Duguet, A. (2014). Will students pass a competitive exam that they failed in their dreams? Consciousness and Cognition, 29, 36–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aumann, C., Lahl, O., & Pietrowsky, R. (2012). Relationship between dream structure, boundary structure and the Big Five personality dimensions. Dreaming, 22(2), 124–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baekeland, F., Resch, R., & Katz, D. (1968). Presleep mentation and dream reports: I. Cognitive style, contiguity to sleep and time of the night. Archives of General Psychiatry, 19, 300–311.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, D. (2001). The committee of sleep: How artists, scientists, and athletes use dreams for creative problem-solving – And how you can too. New York: Crown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bell, A. P., & Hall, C. S. (1971). The personality of a child molester: An analysis of dreams. Chicago: Aldine-Atherton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger, R. J. (1963). Experimental modification of dream content by meaningful verbal stimuli. British Journal of Psychiatry, 109, 722–740.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein, D. M., & Belicki, K. (1995). On the psychometric properties of retrospective dream content questionnaires. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 15, 351–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein, D. M., Belicki, K., & Gonzales, D. (1995). The development and assessment of the reliability and validity of a dream content questionnaire (DCQ). Sleep Research, 25, 138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blagrove, M., Fouquet, N. C., Henley-Einion, J. A., Pace-Schott, E. F., Davies, A. C., Neuschaffer, J. L., & Turnbull, O. H. (2011). Assessing the dream-lag effect for REM and NREM stage 2 dreams. PLoS ONE, 6(10), e26708.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Blagrove, M., Henley-Einion, J., Barnett, A., Edwards, D., & Seage, C. H. (2011). A replication of the 5–7 day dream-lag effect with comparison of dreams to future events as control for baseline matching. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(2), 384–391.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bokert, E. G. (1967). The effect of thirst and a related verbal stimulus on dream reports. Philosophical Dissertation, New York University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Botman, H. I., & Crovitz, H. F. (1989). Dream reports and autobiographical memory. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 9, 213–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Breger, L., Hunter, I., & Lane, R. W. (1971). The effect of stress on dreams. New York: International Universities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broidy, L., & Agnew, R. (1997). Gender and crime: A general strain theory perspective. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 34(3), 275–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruni, O., Lo Reto, F., Recine, A., Ottaviano, S., & Guidetti, V. (1999). Development and validation of a dream content questionnaire for school age children. Sleep and Hypnosis, 1, 41–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, R. A., Wyzenbeek, M., & Weinstein, J. (2011). Dream rebound of suppressed emotional thoughts: The influence of cognitive load. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(3), 515–522.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bulkeley, K., & Domhoff, G. W. (2010). Detecting meaning in dream reports: An extension of a word search approach. Dreaming, 20, 77–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bulkeley, K., & Kahan, T. L. (2008). The impact of September 11 on dreaming. Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 1248–1256.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, K. B., & Muller-Gass, A. (2011). The extent of processing of near-hearing threshold stimuli during natural sleep. Sleep, 34, 1243–1249.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Carskadon, M. A., Wyatt, J., Etgen, G., & Rosekind, M. R. (1989). Nonvisual sensory experiences in dreams of college students. Sleep Research, 18, 159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cartwright, R. D., Bernick, N., Borowitz, G., & Kling, A. (1969). Effect of an erotic movie on the sleep and dreams of young men. Archives of General Psychiatry, 20, 262–271.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Curci, A., & Rime, B. (2008). Dreams, emotions, and social sharing of dreams. Cognition and Emotion, 22, 155–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Jong, M. A., & Visser, P. (1983). Mood, dream content and secondary revision after different pre-sleep stimuli. Sleep Research, 12, 175.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Koninck, J., & Brunette, R. (1991). Presleep suggestion related to a phobic object: Successful manipulation of reported dream affect. Journal of General Psychology, 118, 185–200.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Koninck, J., & Koulack, D. (1975). Dream content and adaption to a stressful situation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 84, 250–260.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeDonato, A., Belicki, K., & Cuddy, M. (1996). Rater’s abilities to identify individuals reporting sexual abuse from nightmare content. Dreaming, 6, 33–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dement, W. C., & Wolpert, E. A. (1958). The relation of eye movements, body motility and external stimuli to dream content. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44, 543–553.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Domhoff, G. W. (2003). The scientific study of dreams: Neural networks, cognitive development and content analysis. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Domhoff, G. W. (2017). The invasion of the concept snatchers: The origins, distortions, and future of the continuity hypothesis. Dreaming, 27(1), 14–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Domhoff, G. W. (2018). The emergence of dreaming: Mind-wandering, embodied simulation, and the default network. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Domhoff, G. W., Meyer-Gomes, K., & Schredl, M. (2005–2006). Dreams as the expression of conceptions and concerns: A comparison of German and American college students. Imagination, Cognition & Personality, 25, 269–282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duffey, T. H., Wooten, H. R., Lamadue, C. A., & Comstock, D. C. (2004). The effects of dream sharing on marital intimacy and satisfaction. Journal of Couple and Relationship Therapy, 3, 53–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H. (1987). Sex differences in social behavior: A social-role interpretation. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., & Steffen, V. J. (1986). Gender and aggressive behavior: A meta-analytic review of the social psychology literature. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 309–330.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Erdelyi, M. H. (2017). The continuity hypothesis. Dreaming, 27(4), 334–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erlacher, D., & Schredl, M. (2004). Dreams reflecting waking sport activities: A comparison of sport and psychology students. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 35, 301–308.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fosse, M. J., Fosse, R., Hobson, J. A., & Stickgold, R. J. (2003). Dreaming and episodic memory: A functional dissociation? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 1–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Foulkes, D., & Griffin, M. L. (1976). An experimental study of “creative dreaming”. Sleep Research, 5, 129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foulkes, D., & Rechtschaffen, A. (1964). Presleep determinants of dream content. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 19, 983–1005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Foulkes, D., Pivik, T., Steadman, H. S., Spear, P. S., & Symonds, J. D. (1967). Dreams of the male child: An EEG study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 72, 457–467.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. (1900/1991). The interpretation of dreams (Org.: Die Traumdeutung). London: Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gackenbach, J., Rosie, M., Bown, J., & Sample, T. (2011). Dream incorporation of video-game play as a function of interactivity and fidelity. Dreaming, 21(1), 32–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Georgi, M., Schredl, M., Henley-Einion, J., & Blagrove, M. (2012). Gender differences in dreaming in childhood and adolescence: The UK library study. International Journal of Dream Research, 5, 125–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodenough, D. R., Witkin, H. A., Koulack, D., & Cohen, H. (1975). The effects of stress films on dream affect and on respiration and eye movement activity during rapid-eye-movement sleep. Psychophysiology, 12, 313–320.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grenier, J., Cappeliez, P., St-Onge, M., Vachon, J., Vinette, S., Roussy, F., Mercier, P., Lortie-Lussier, M., & De Koninck, J. (2005). Temporal references in dreams and autobiographical memory. Memory and Cognition, 33, 280–288.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, M. L., & Foulkes, D. (1977). Deliberate presleep control of dream content: An experimental study. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 45, 660–662.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Griffith, R. M., Miyagi, O., & Tago, A. (1958). The universality of typical dreams: Japanese vs. Americans. American Anthropologist, 60, 1173–1179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gruber, R. E. (1988). Dreaming style and waking personality. Unpublished Dissertation, University of Cincinnati.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, C. S. (1947). Diagnosing personality by the analysis of dreams. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 42, 68–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, C. S. (1967). Representation of the laboratory setting in dreams. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 144, 198–206.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, C. S. (1984). “A ubiquitous sex difference in dreams” revisited. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 1109–1117.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, C. S., & Domhoff, B. J. (1963). A ubiquitous sex difference in dreams. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66, 278–280.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, C. S., & Nordby, V. J. (1972). The individual and his dreams. New York: New American Library.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, C. S., & Van de Castle, R. L. (1966). The content analysis of dreams. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, C. S., Domhoff, G. W., Blick, K. A., & Weesner, K. E. (1982). The dreams of college men and women in 1959 and 1980: A comparison of dream contents and sex differences. Sleep, 5, 188–194.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hartmann, E. (2000). We do not dream of the 3 R’s: implications for the nature of dream mentation. Dreaming, 10, 103–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hartmann, E. (2010). The underlying emotion and the dream: Relating dream imagery to the dreamer’s underlying emotion can help elucidate the nature of dreaming. International Review of Neurobiology, 92, 197–214.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hartmann, E. (2011). The nature and functions of dreaming. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartmann, E., & Basile, R. (2003). Dream imagery becomes more intense after 9/11/01. Dreaming, 13, 61–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hearne, K. M. T. (1978). Lucid dreams: An electrophysiological and psychological study. Doctoral dissertation, University of Liverpool.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heermann, G. (1838). Beobachtungen und Betrachtungen über die Träume der Blinden, ein Beitrag zur Physiologie und Psychologie der Sinne. Monatsschrift für Medicin, Augenheilkunde und Chirurgie, 1, 116–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henley-Einion, J. A., & Blagrove, M. T. (2014). Assessing the day-residue and dream-lag effects using the identification of multiple correspondences between dream reports and waking life diaries. Dreaming, 24(2), 71–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, J. A. (1988). The dreaming brain. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, J. A., & McCarley, R. W. (1977). The brain as a dream state generator: An activation-synthesis hypothesis of the dream process. American Journal of Psychiatry, 134, 1335–1348.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, J. A., & Voss, U. (2011). A mind to go out of: Reflections on primary and secondary consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 993–997.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, J. A., Hoffman, S. A., Helfand, R., & Kostner, D. (1987). Dream bizarreness and the activation-synthesis hypothesis. Human Neurobiology, 6, 157–164.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, J. A., Pace-Schott, E. F., & Stickgold, R. (2000). Dreaming and the brain: Toward a cognitive neuroscience of conscious states. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 793–842.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoelscher, T. J., Klinger, E., & Barta, S. G. (1981). Incorporation of concern- and nonconcern-related verbal stimuli into dream content. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90, 88–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ibanez, A. M., Martin, R. S., Hurtado, E., & Lopez, V. (2009). ERPs studies of cognitive processing during sleep. International Journal of Psychology, 44(4), 290–304.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jung, C. G. (1979). Allgemeine Gesichtspunkte zur Psychologie des Traumes. In C. G. Jung (Ed.), Gesammelte Werke Band 8: Die Dynamik des Unbewußten (pp. 268–308). Olten: Walter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahan, T. L., & Claudatos, S. (2016). Phenomenological features of dreams: Results from dream log studies using the Subjective Experiences Rating Scale (SERS). Consciousness and Cognition, 41, 159–176.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kahan, T. L., LaBerge, S., Levitan, L., & Zimbardo, P. (1997). Similarities and differences between waking and dreaming cognition: An exploratory study. Consciousness and Cognition, 6, 132–147.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kahn, E., Dement, W., Fisher, C., & Barmack, J. E. (1962). Incidence of color in immediately recalled dreams. Science, 137, 1054–1055.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kallmeyer, R. J., & Chang, E. C. (1997). The multidimensional dream inventory: Preliminary evidence for validity and reliability. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 85, 803–808.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kern, S., Auer, A., Gutsche, M., Otto, A., Preuß, K., & Schredl, M. (2014). Relation between waking politic, music and sports related tasks and dream content in students of politics and psychology students. International Journal of Dream Research, 7, 80–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kerr, N. H., & Domhoff, G. W. (2004). Do the blind literally “see” in their dreams? A critique of a recent claim that they do. Dreaming, 14, 230–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kerr, N. H., Foulkes, D., & Schmidt, M. (1982). The structure of laboratory dream reports in blind and sighted subjects. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 170, 286–294.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • König, N., Heizmann, L. M., Göritz, A. S., & Schredl, M. (2017). Colors in dreams and the introduction of color TV in Germany: An online study. International Journal of Dream Research, 10, 59–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Köthe, M., & Pietrowsky, R. (2001). Behavioral effects of nightmares and their correlations to personality patterns. Dreaming, 11, 43–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koulack, D. (1969). Effects of somatosensory stimulation on dream content. Archives of General Psychiatry, 20, 718–725.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krakow, B., Tandberg, D., Barey, M., & Scriggins, L. (1995). Nightmares and sleep disturbance in sexually assaulted women. Dreaming, 5, 199–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kramer, M. (2007). The dream experience: A systematic exploration. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kramer, M., Schoen, L. S., & Kinney, L. (1984). The dream experience in dream-disturbed Vietnam veterans. In B. A. Van der Kolk (Ed.), Posttraumatic stress disorder: Psychological and biological sequelace (pp. 82–95). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kröner-Borowik, T., Gosch, S., Hansen, K., Borowik, B., Schredl, M., & Steil, R. (2013). The effects of suppressing intrusive thoughts on dream content, dream distress and psychological parameters. Journal of Sleep Research, 22(5), 600–604.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kroth, J., Gilbert, H., Guichard, A., & Quatman, T. (1999). Analysis of factor structure in a dream inventory. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 89, 657–658.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, S. P. (1985). Lucid dreaming. Los Angeles: Jeremy P. Tarcher.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lambrecht, S., Schredl, M., Henley-Einion, J., & Blagrove, M. (2013). Self-rated effects of reading, TV viewing and daily activities on dreaming in adolescents and adults: The UK library study. International Journal of Dream Research, 6, 41–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lauer, C., Riemann, D., Lund, R., & Berger, M. (1987). Shortened REM latency: A consequence of psychological strain? Psychophysiology, 24, 263–271.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, K., & Ogilvie, R. (1996). Vestibular dreams: The effect of rocking on dream mentation. Dreaming, 6, 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lortie-Lussier, M., Simond, S., Rinfret, N., & De Koninck, J. (1992). Beyond sex differences: Family and occupational roles’ impact on women’s and men’s dreams. Sex Roles, 26, 79–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malinowski, J. E., & Horton, C. L. (2014a). Evidence for the preferential incorporation of emotional waking-life experiences into dreams. Dreaming, 24(1), 18–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malinowski, J. E., & Horton, C. L. (2014b). Memory sources of dreams: The incorporation of autobiographical rather than episodic experiences. Journal of Sleep Research, 23, 441–447.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marquardt, C. J., Bonato, R. A., & Hoffmann, R. F. (1996). An empirical investigation into day-residue and dream-lag effects. Dreaming, 6, 57–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mathes, J., & Schredl, M. (2013). Gender differences in dream content: Are they related to personality? International Journal of Dream Research, 6, 104–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maury, A. (1861). Le sommeil et les reves. Paris: Didier.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarley, R. W., & Hoffman, E. (1981). REM sleep dreams and the activation-synthesis hypothesis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 138, 904–912.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meier, B. (1993). Speech and thinking in dream. In C. Cavallero & D. Foulkes (Eds.), Dreaming as cognition (pp. 58–76). London: Harvester Wheatsheaf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mellman, T. A., David, D., Bustamante, V., Torres, J., & Fins, A. (2001). Dreams in the acute aftermath of trauma and their relationship to PTSD. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 14, 241–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meumann, E. (1909). Über Lesen und Schreiben im Traume. Archiv für die Gesamte Psychologie, 15, 380–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, T. A. (1993). Changes in the kinesthetic content of dreams following somatosensory stimulation of leg muscles during REM sleep. Dreaming, 3, 99–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, T. A., & Powell, R. A. (1989). The ‘dream-lag’ effect: A 6-day temporal delay in dream content incorporation. Psychiatric Journal of the University of Ottawa, 14, 561–565.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, T. A., & Powell, R. A. (1992). The day-residue and dream lag effects: A literature review and limited replication of two temporal effects in dream formation. Dreaming, 2, 67–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, T. A., McGregor, D. L., Zadra, A. L., Ilnicki, D., & Ouellet, L. (1993). Pain in dreams. Sleep, 16, 490–498.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, T. A., Kuiken, D., Alain, G., Stenstrom, P., & Powell, R. A. (2004). Immediate and delayed incorporations of events into dreams: Further replication and implication for dream function. Journal of Sleep Research, 13, 327–336.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oliver, M. B., & Hyde, J. S. (1993). Gender differences in sexuality: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 29–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Olsen, M. R., Schredl, M., & Carlsson, I. (2013). Sharing dreams: Frequency, motivations, and relationship intimacy. Dreaming, 23(4), 245–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paul, F., & Schredl, M. (2012). Male-female ratio in waking-life contacts and dream characters. International Journal of Dream Research, 5, 119–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paul, F., Schädlich, M., & Erlacher, D. (2014). Lucid dream induction by visual and tactile stimulation: An exploratory sleep laboratory study. International Journal of Dream Research, 7(1), 61–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pigman, G. W. (2002). The dark forest of authors: Freud and nineteenth-century dream theory. Psychoanalysis and History, 4, 141–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Powell, R. A., Nielsen, T. A., Cheung, J. S., & Cervenka, T. M. (1995). Temporal delays in incorporation of events into dreams. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 81, 95–104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Purcell, S., Mullington, J., Moffit, A., Hoffmann, R., & Pigeau, R. (1986). Dream self-reflectiveness as a learned cognitive skill. Sleep, 9(3), 423–437.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rahimi, S., Naghibi, S. M., Mokhber, N., Schredl, M., Assadpour, H., Ramezani Farkhani, A., Nemati Karimoui, H. A., Mohajeri, S. M. R., Darvish, A., Naghibi, S. S., & Sadjadi, S. A. (2015). Sophisticated evaluation of possible effect of distinct auditory stimulation during REM sleep on dream content. International Journal of Dream Research, 8(2), 146–151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rechtschaffen, A. (1978). The single-mindedness and isolation of dreams. Sleep, 1, 97–109.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rechtschaffen, A., & Buchignani, C. (1992). The visual appearance of dreams. In J. S. Antrobus & M. Bertini (Eds.), The neuropsychology of sleep and dreaming (pp. 143–155). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rechtschaffen, A., & Foulkes, D. (1965). Effect of visual stimuli on dream content. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 20, 1149–1160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, H. (1978). Meditation and lucid dreaming: A statistical relationship. Sundance Community Dream Journal, 2, 237–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Revonsuo, A. (2000). The reinterpretation of dreams: An evolutionary hypothesis of the function of dreaming. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 877–901.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Revonsuo, A., Tuominen, J., & Valli, K. (2015). The Avatars in the machine. In T. K. Metzinger & J. M. Windt (Eds.), Open mind (pp. 1–28). Frankfurt am Main: Mind Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roffwarg, H. P., Herman, J. H., Bowe-Anders, C., & Tauber, E. S. (1978). The effects of sustained alterations of waking visual input on dream content. In A. M. Arkin, J. S. Antrobus, & S. J. Ellman (Eds.), The mind in sleep: Psychology and psychophysiology (pp. 295–349). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roussy, F., Raymond, I., Gonthier, I., Grenier, J., & De Koninck, J. (1998). Temporal references in manifest dream content: Confirmation of increased remoteness as the night progresses. Sleep Supplement, 21, 285.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sandman, N., Valli, K., Kronholm, E., Ollila, H. M., Revonsuo, A., Laatikainen, T., & Paunio, T. (2013). Nightmares: Prevalence among the Finnish general adult population and war veterans during 1972–2007. Sleep, 36, 1041–1050.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, R. E., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2008). Dreaming of white bears: The return of the suppressed at sleep onset. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(3), 714–724.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M. (2000a). Continuity between waking life and dreaming: Are all waking activities reflected equally often in dreams. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 90, 844–846.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M. (2000b). The effect of dreams on waking life. Sleep and Hypnosis, 2, 120–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M. (2001). Dreams of singles: Effects of waking-life social contacts on dream content. Personality and Individual Differences, 31, 269–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M. (2002). Questionnaire and diaries as research instruments in dream research: Methodological issues. Dreaming, 12, 17–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M. (2003). Continuity between waking and dreaming: A proposal for a mathematical model. Sleep and Hypnosis, 5, 38–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M. (2006). Factors affecting the continuity between waking and dreaming: Emotional intensity and emotional tone of the waking-life event. Sleep and Hypnosis, 8, 1–5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M. (2008a). Determining the dreamer’s gender from a single dream report: A matching study. International Journal of Dream Research, 1, 23–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M. (2008b). Laboratory references in dreams: Methodological problem and/or evidence for the continuity hypothesis of dreaming? International Journal of Dream Research, 1, 3–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M. (2008c). Personality correlates of flying dreams. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 27, 129–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M. (2009a). Effect of dreams on daytime mood: The effects of gender and personality. Sleep and Hypnosis, 11, 51–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M. (2009b). Sharing dreams: Sex and other sociodemographic variables. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 109, 235–238.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M. (2011a). Frequency and nature of pain in a long dream series. Sleep and Hypnosis, 13, 1–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M. (2011b). Reading dream literature: Frequency, influencing factors, and self-rated benefit. American Journal of Psychology, 124, 227–233.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M. (2012). Continuity in studying the continuity hypothesis of dreaming is needed. International Journal of Dream Research, 5, 1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M. (2015). Dreaming about dreaming: Analysis of a long dream series. International Journal of Dream Research, 8, 142–145.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M. (2016). Temperature perception in dreams: Analysis of a long dream series. International journal of dream research, 9, 79–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M. (2017). Theorizing about the continuity between waking and dreaming: Comment on Domhoff (2017). Dreaming, 27(4), 351–359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M. (2018). Reminiscences of love: Former romantic partners in dreams. International Journal of Dream Research, 11(1), 69–73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., & Engelhardt, H. (2001). Dreaming and psychopathology: Dream recall and dream content of psychiatric inpatients. Sleep and Hypnosis, 3, 44–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., & Erlacher, D. (2003). The problem of dream content analysis validity as shown by a bizarreness scale. Sleep and Hypnosis, 5, 129–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., & Erlacher, D. (2007). Self-reported effects of dreams on waking-life creativity: An empirical study. Journal of Psychology, 141, 35–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., & Erlacher, D. (2008). Relationship between waking sport activities, reading and dream content in sport and psychology students. Journal of Psychology, 142, 267–275.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., & Göritz, A. S. (2014). Umgang mit Alpträumen in der Allgemeinbevölkerung: Eine Online-Studie. [Coping with nightmares in the general population: An online study]. Psychotherapie, Psychosomatik und Medizinische Psychologie, 64(5), 192–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., & Hofmann, F. (2003). Continuity between waking activities and dream activities. Consciousness and Cognition, 12, 298–308.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., & Keller, K. (2008–2009). Dream content in a representative German sample: Gender differences and the effects of other socio-demographic variables. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 28, 37–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., & Pallmer, R. (1998). Geschlechtsunterschiede in Angstträumen von SchülerInnen. Praxis der Kinderpsychologie und Kinderpsychiatrie, 47, 463–476.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., & Piel, E. (2004). War-related dream themes in Germany from 1956 to 2000. Journal of Sleep Research Supplement, 13(1), 658.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., & Piel, E. (2005). Gender differences in dreaming: Are they stable over time? Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 309–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., & Reinhard, I. (2008). Gender differences in dream recall: A meta-analysis. Journal of Sleep Research, 17, 125–131.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., & Reinhard, I. (2009–2010). The continuity between waking mood and dream emotions: Direct and second-order effects. Imagination, Cognition and Personality 29, 271-282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., & Reinhard, I. (2012). Frequency of a romantic partner in a dream series. Dreaming, 22(4), 223–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., & Schawinski, J. A. (2010). Frequency of dream sharing: The effects of gender and personality. American Journal of Psychology, 123, 93–101.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., Kleinferchner, P., & Gell, T. (1996). Dreaming and personality: Thick vs. thin boundaries. Dreaming, 6, 219–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., Sahin, V., & Schäfer, G. (1998). Gender differences in dreams: Do they reflect gender differences in waking life? Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 433–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., Schäfer, G., Hofmann, F., & Jacob, S. (1999). Dream content and personality: Thick vs. thin boundaries. Dreaming, 9, 257–263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., Ciric, P., Bishop, A., Gölitz, E., & Buschtöns, D. (2003). Content analysis of German students’ dreams: Comparison to American findings. Dreaming, 13, 237–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., Schwenger, C., & Dehe, A. (2004). Gender differences in dreams: A matching study. Counseling and Clinical Psychology Journal, 1, 61–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., Fuchedzhieva, A., Hämig, H., & Schindele, V. (2008). Do we think dreams are in black and white due to memory problems? Dreaming, 18, 175–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., Atanasova, D., Hörmann, K., Maurer, J. T., Hummel, T., & Stuck, B. A. (2009). Information processing during sleep: The effect of olfactory stimuli on dream content and dream emotions. Journal of Sleep Research, 18, 285–290.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., Bocklage, A., Engelhardt, J., & Mingebach, T. (2009). Psychological boundaries, dream recall, and nightmare frequency: A new Boundary Personality Questionnaire (BPQ). International Journal of Dream Research, 2, 12–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., Desch, S., Röming, F., & Spachmann, A. (2009). Erotic dreams and their relationship to waking-life sexuality. Sexologies, 18, 38–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., Hebel, M. E., Klütsch, R. C., & Liehe, L. J. (2009). The role of mood congruency memory effects in dream recall: A pilot study. Dreaming, 19, 113–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., Riemann, D., & Berger, M. (2009). The effect of trimipramine on dream recall and dream emotions in depressive outpatients. Psychiatry Research, 167, 279–286.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., Becker, K., & Feldman, E. (2010). Predicting the dreamer’s gender from a single dream report: A matching study in a non-student sample. International Journal of Dream Research, 3, 130–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., Paul, F., Lahl, O., & Göritz, A. S. (2010–2011). Gender differences in dream content: Related to biological sex or sex role orientation? Imagination, Cognition, and Personality, 30, 171–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., Berres, S., Klingauf, A., Schellhaas, S., & Göritz, A. S. (2014). The Mannheim Dream questionnaire (MADRE): Retest reliability, age and gender effects. International Journal of Dream Research, 7, 141–147.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., Hoffmann, L., Sommer, J. U., & Stuck, B. A. (2014). Olfactory stimulation during sleep can reactivate odor-associated images. Chemosensory Perception, 7, 140–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., Buscher, A., Haaß, C., Scheuermann, M., & Uhrig, K. (2015). Gender differences in dream socialisation in children and adolescents. International Journal of Adolescence and Youth, 20, 61–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., Kim, E., Labudek, S., Schädler, A., & Göritz, A. S. (2015). Factors affecting the gender difference in dream sharing frequency. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 34(3), 306–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schredl, M., Kälberer, A., Zacharowski, K., & Zimmermann, M. (2017). Pain dreams and dream emotions in patients with chronic back pain and healthy controls. Open Pain Journal, 10, 65–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwitzgebel, E. (2002). Why did we think we dreamed in black and white? Studies in History and Philosophy of Science, 33, 649–660.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Selterman, D., Apetroaia, A., & Waters, E. (2012). Script-like attachment representations in dreams containing current romantic partners. Attachment & Human Development, 14(5), 501–515.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sikka, P., Feilhauer, D., Valli, K., & Revonsuo, A. (2017). How you measure is what you get: Differences in self- and external ratings of emotional experiences in home dreams. American Journal of Psychology, 130(3), 367–384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snyder, F. (1970). The phenomenology of dreaming. In L. Madow & L. H. Snow (Eds.), The psychodynamic implications of the physiological studies on dreams (pp. 124–151). Springfield: Charles C. Thomas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Soper, B., Milford, G. E., & Rosenthal, G. T. (1994). Dream perspective: A research note. Journal of Mental Imagery, 18, 181–182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, D. W., & Koulack, D. (1993). The functions of dreams in adaption to stress over time. Dreaming, 3, 259–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stickgold, R., Malia, A., Maguire, D., Roddenberry, D., & O’Connor, M. (2000). Replaying the game: Hypnagogic images in normals and amnesics. Science, 290, 350–353.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stickgold, R., Hobson, J. A., Fosse, R., & Fosse, M. J. (2001). Sleep, learning, and dreams: Off-line memory reprocessing. Science, 294, 1052–1057.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strauch, I., & Meier, B. (1996). In search of dreams: Results of experimental dream research. Albany: State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stumbrys, T. (2011). Lucid dreaming: Discontinuity or continuity in consciousness? International Journal of Dream Research, 4, 93–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stumbrys, T., & Erlacher, D. (2016). Mindfulness and lucid dream frequency predicts the ability to control lucid dreams. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 36(3), 229–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stumbrys, T., Erlacher, D., Johnson, M., & Schredl, M. (2014). The phenomenology of lucid dreaming: An online survey. American Journal of Psychology, 127(2), 191–204.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stumbrys, T., Erlacher, D., & Malinowski, P. (2015). Meta-awareness during day and night: The relationship between mindfulness and lucid dreaming. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 34(4), 415–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Takeuchi, T., Ogilvie, R. D., Ferrelli, A. V., Murphy, T. I., & Belicki, K. (2001). The dream property scale: An exploratory English version. Consciousness and Cognition, 10, 341–355.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tartz, R. S., & Krippner, S. (2017). Cognitive differences in dream content between Japanese males and females using quantitative content analysis. Dreaming, 27(3), 193–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, F., & Bryant, R. A. (2007). The tendency to suppress, inhibiting thoughts, and dream rebound. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 163–168.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Terr, L. C. (1981). Psychic trauma in children: Observations following the Chowchilla school-bus kidnapping. American Journal of Psychiatry, 138, 14–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Terr, L. C. (1983). Chowchilla revisited: The effects of psychic trauma four years after a schoolbus kidnapping. American Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 1543–1550.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trotter, K., Dallas, K., & Verdone, P. (1988). Olfactory stimuli and their effects on REM dreams. Psychiatric Journal of the University of Ottawa, 13, 94–96.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vallat, R., Chatard, B., Blagrove, M., & Ruby, P. (2017). Characteristics of the memory sources of dreams: A new version of the content-matching paradigm to take mundane and remote memories into account. PLoS One, 12(10), e0185262.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Van den Bulck, J., Çetin, Y., Terzi, Ö., & Bushman, B. J. (2016). Violence, sex, and dreams: Violent and sexual media content infiltrate our dreams at night. Dreaming, 26(4), 271–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Rijn, E., Eichenlaub, J. B., Lewis, P. A., Walker, M. P., Gaskell, M. G., Malinowski, J. E., & Blagrove, M. (2015). The dream-lag effect: Selective processing of personally significant events during Rapid Eye Movement sleep, but not during Slow Wave Sleep. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 122, 98–109.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Verdone, P. (1965). Temporal reference of manifest dream content. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 20, 1253–1268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vogelsang, L., Anold, S., Schormann, J., Wübbelmann, S., & Schredl, M. (2016). The continuity between waking-life musical activities and music dreams. Dreaming, 26(2), 132–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Voss, U., Tuin, I., Schermelleh-Engel, K., & Hobson, J. A. (2011). Waking and dreaming: Related but structurally independent. Dream reports of congenitally paraplegic and deaf-mute persons. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(3), 673–687.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M., Wenzlaff, R. M., & Kozak, M. (2004). Dream rebound: The return of suppressed thoughts in dreams. Psychological Science, 15, 232–236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wittmann, L., Schredl, M., & Kramer, M. (2007). The role of dreaming in posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 76, 25–39.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wittmann, L., Zehnder, D., Schredl, M., Jenni, O. G., & Landolt, M. A. (2010). Posttraumatic nightmares and psychopathology in children after road traffic accidents. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 23, 232–239.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yu, C. K.-C. (2012). Dream Motif Scale. Dreaming, 22(1), 18–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zadra, A. L., Nielsen, T. A., & Donderi, D. C. (1997). The prevalence of auditory, olfactory, gustatory and pain experiences in 3372 home dreams. Sleep Research, 26, 281.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Schredl, M. (2018). Dream Behavior and Dream Content in Healthy Persons. In: Researching Dreams. Palgrave Macmillan, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-95453-0_4

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics