Skip to main content

Development of Motivation

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Abstract

From its earliest development on human behavior is characterized by a striving for control. As humans develop, this striving becomes more concrete, goal-oriented, and reflective. Children’s daily interaction partners play a crucial role in the development of fundamental motivational preferences and behavioral regulation. The behavioral emotions of pride and shame indicate self-evaluation and thus become positive or negative incentives for future behavior. Children’s gradual cognitive development allows them to gain a more elaborate understanding of task difficulty, abilities, effort, and their complex interactions for the prediction and explanation of behavioral outcomes. In addition to general developmental trends, people develop individual differences in their implicit motives, specific incentives and expectancies, generalized performance goal orientations, as well as strengths and weaknesses in behavioral regulation. These individual differences become increasingly pronounced at crucial developmental transitions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Notes

  1. 1.

    The term “competence” is used as a summary construct comprising both ability and effort.

References

  • Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49–79.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adams, R. E., & Passman, R. H. (1979). Effects of visual and auditory aspects of mothers and stranger on the play and exploration of children. Developmental Psychology, 15, 269–274.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ainley, M. D. (1993). Styles of engagement with learning: Multidimensional assessment of their relationship with strategy use and school achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 395–405.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ainsworth, M. D. (1972). Attachment and dependency: A comparison. In J. Gewirtz (Ed.), Attachment and dependency. Washington, DC: Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ainsworth, M. D., & Bell, S. M. (1970). Attachment, exploration and separation: Illustrated by the behavior of one-year-olds in a strange situation. Child Development, 41, 49–67.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ainsworth, M. D., Bell, S. M., & Stayton, D. J. (1974). Infant mother attachment and social development: Socialisation as a product of reciprocal responsiveness to signals. In R. P. M. Richards (Ed.), The integration of a child into a social world. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Altshuber, J. A., & Ruble, D. N. (1989). Developmental changes in children’s awareness of strategies for coping with uncontrollable stress. Child Development, 60, 1337–1349.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ames, C. (1978). Childrens achievement attributions and self reinforcement: Effects of self concept and competitive reward structure. Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, 345–355.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ames, C., Ames, R., & Felker, D. W. (1977). Effects of competitive reward structure and valence of outcome on children’s achievement attributions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ames, C., & Archer, J. (1988). Achievement goals in the classroom: Students learning strategies and motivational processes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 260–267.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, J. W. (1957). Motivational determinants of risk-taking behavior. Psychological Review, 64, 359–372.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, J. W. (1964). Some neglected variables in contemporary conceptions of decision and performance. Psychological Reports, 14, 575–590.

    Google Scholar 

  • Band, E. B., & Weisz, J. R. (1988). How to feel better when it feels bad: Children’s perspectives on coping with everyday stress. Developmental Psychology, 24, 246–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, K. C., & Morgan, G. A. (1995). Continuities and discontinuities in mastery motivation during infancy and toddlerhood: A conceptualization and review. In R. H. MacTurk & G. A. Morgan (Eds.), Mastery motivation: Origins, conceptualization and applications (pp. 57–93). Westport, CT: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, K. C., Morgan, G. A., & Maslin-Cole, B. (1993). Three studies on the development of mastery motivation in infancy and toddlerhood. In D. Messer (Ed.), Mastery motivation in early childhood: Development, measurement and social processes (pp. 83–108). London, UK: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barron, K. E., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2001). Achievement goals and optimal motivation: Testing multiple goal models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 706–722.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bartlett, E. W., & Smith, C. P. (1966). Childrearing practices, birth order and the development of achievement-related motives. Psychological Reports, 19, 1207–1216.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beneson, J., & Dweck, C. S. (1986). The development of trait explanation and self-evaluations in the academic and social domains. Child Development, 57, 1179–1189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bindman, S. W., Pomerantz, E. M., & Roisman, G. I. (2015). Do children’s executive functions account for associations between early autonomy-supportive parenting and achievement through high school? Journal of Educational Psychology, 107, 756–770.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Brandtstädter, J. (2001). Entwicklung, Intentionalität, Handeln. Stuttgart, Germany: Kohlhammer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (1988). The ecology of developmental processes. In W. Damon & M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. Bd. 1, pp. 993–1028). New York, NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brunstein, J. C., & Hoyer, S. (2002). Implizites versus explizites Leistungsstreben: Befunde zur Unabhängigkeit zweier Motivationssysteme. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 16, 51–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brunstein, J. C., & Maier, G. W. (1996). Persönliche Ziele: Ein Überblick zum Stand der Forschung. Psychologische Rundschau, 47, 146–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bühler, K. (1922). Die geistige Entwicklung des Kindes (3. Aufl ed.). Jena, Germany: Fischer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bullock, M., & Lütkenhaus, P. (1988). The development of volitional behavior in the toddler years. Child Development, 59, 664–674.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bullock, M., & Lütkenhaus, P. (1991). Who am I? Self understanding in toddlers. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 36, 217–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butler, R. (1999). Information seeking and achievement motivation in middle childhood and adolescence: The role of conceptions of ability. Developmental Psychology, 35, 146–163.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carr, S. J., Dabbs, J. M., & Carr, G. (1975). Mother-infant attachment: The importance of the mother’s visual field. Child Development, 46, 331–338.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (1973). Interactions between mothers and their young children: Characteristics and consequences. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38(153), 6–7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clarke-Stewart, K. A., VanderStoep, L. P., & Killian, P. (1979). Analysis and replication of mother-child relations at two years of age. Child Development, 50, 777–793.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collis, G. M., & Schaffer, H. R. (1975). Synchronization of visual attention in mother-infant pairs. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 16, 315–320.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Compas, B. E., Connor-Smith, J. K., Saltzman, H., Thomasen, A. H., & Wadsworth, M. E. (2001). Coping with stress during childhood and adolescence: Problems, progress, and potential in theory and research. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 87–127.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Covington, M. V., & Omelich, C. L. (1979). Are arousal attributions causal? A path analysis of the cognitive model of achievement motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1487–1504.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond boredom and anxiety. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. (deutsch: Das Flow-Erlebnis. Stuttgart: Klett-Cotta, 1999, 8. Aufl.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Daniels, L. M., Haynes, T. L., Stupnisky, R. H., Perry, R. P., Newall, N. E., & Pekrun, R. (2008). Individual differences in achievement goals: A longitudinal study of cognitive, emotional, and achievement outcomes. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 33, 584–608.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeCasper, A. J., & Carstens, A. A. (1981). Contingencies of stimulation: Effects of learning and emotion in neonates. Infant Behavior and Development. Special Issue: Variability in Infancy, 4, 19–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diener, C. I., & Dweck, C. S. (1978). An analysis of learned helplessness: Continuous changes in performance, strategy, and achievement cognitions following failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 451–462.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diener, C. I., & Dweck, C. S. (1980). An analysis of learned helplessness: II. The processing of success. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 940–952.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DiVitto, B., & McArthur, L. Z. (1978). Developmental differences in the use of distinctiveness, consensus, and consistency information for making causal attributions. Developmental Psychology, 14, 474–482.

    Google Scholar 

  • Droege, K. L., & Stipek, D. J. (1993). Children’s use of dispositions to predict classmates’ behavior. Developmental Psychology, 29, 646–654.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S. (1975). The role of expectations and attributions in the alleviation of learned helplessness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 674–685.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. In Essays in social psychology, 195. New York, NY: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S. (2002). The development of ability conceptions. In A. Wigfield & J. S. Eccles (Eds.), Development of achievement motivation (pp. 57–88). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S. (2003). Clarifying achievement goals and their impact. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 541–553.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, F. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256–273.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S., & Repucci, N. D. (1973). Learned helplessness and reinforcement responsibility in children. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 25, 109–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J. S. (1984). Sex differences in achievement patterns. In T. Sonderegger (Ed.), Nebraska symposium of motivation (Vol. 32, pp. 97–132). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J. S. (1987). Gender roles and women’s achievement-related decisions. Psychological Women WQ, 11, 135–172.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J. S. (2005). Subjective task value and the Eccles et al. model of achievement related choices. Handbook of competence and motivation. New York, NY: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J. S., Adler, T. F., & Meece, J. L. (1984). Sex differences in achievement: A test of alternate theories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 26–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J. S., Barber, B., & Jozefowicz, D. (1999). Linking gender to educational, occupational and recreational choices: Applying the Eccles model of achievement related choices. In W. B. Swann & J. H. Langlois (Eds.), Sexism and stereotypes in modern society (pp. 153–192). Washington, DC: APA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J. S., & Wang, M.-T. (2016). What motivates females and males to pursue careers in mathematics and science? International Journal of Behavioral Development, 40, 100–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values, and goals. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 109–132.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., & Schiefele, U. (1998). Motivation to succeed. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 3, 5th ed., pp. 1017–1095). New York, NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eckhardt, G. (1968). Die entwicklungspsychologische Abhängigkeit der Konfliktreaktion vom Grad der Misserfolgswahrscheinlichkeit. Vordiplomarbeit, Psychologisches Institut, Universität Münster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, A. J. (1997). Integrating the “classic” and “contemporary” approaches to achievement motivation: A hierarchical model of achievement motivation. In M. Maehr & P. Pintrich (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 10, pp. 243–279). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, A. J. (1999). Approach and avoidance motivation and achievement goals. Educational Psychologist, 34, 169–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, A. J. (2005). A conceptual history of the achievement goal construct. In A. J. Elliot & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 52–72). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, A. J. (Ed.). (2008). Handbook of approach and avoidance motivation. New York, NY: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, A. J., & Church, M. (1997). A hierarchical model of approach and avoidance achievement motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 218–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, A. J., & McGregor, H. A. (2001). A 2×2 achievement goal framework. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 501–519.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Entwistle, D., & Hayduk, L. (1978). Too great expectations: Young children’s academic outlook. Baltimore, MD: John Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Falbo, T. (1975). Achievement attributions of kindergarteners. Developmental Psychology, 11, 529–530.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farr, J. L., Hofmann, D. A., & Mathieu, J. E. (1993). Job perception, job satisfaction relations: An empirical comparison of three competing theories. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision, 56, 370–387.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feld, S. C. (1967). Longitudinal study of the origins of achievement strivings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7, 408–414.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Finkelstein, N. W., & Ramey, C. T. (1977). Learning to control the environment in infancy. Child Development, 48, 806–819.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, K., Goudas, M., Biddle, S., Duda, J. L., & Armstrong, N. (1994). Children’s task and ego goal profiles in sport. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 64, 253–261.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Franz, E. E. (1994). Does thought content change as individuals age? A longitudinal study of midlife adults. In T. F. Heatherthon & J. L. Weinberger (Eds.), Can personality change? (pp. 227–249). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frey, K. S., & Ruble, D. N. (1985). What children say when the teacher is not around: Conflicting goals in social comparison and performance assessment in the classroom. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 550–562.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N. H. (1988). The laws of emotion. American Psychologist, 43, 249–358.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garon, N., Bryson, S. E., & Smith, I. M. (2008). Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 31–60.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geppert, U., & Gartmann, D. (1983). The emergence of self-evaluative emotions as consequences of achievement actions. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 6, 355–369.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geppert, U., & Heckhausen, H. (1990). Ontogenese der Emotion. In K. R. Scherer (Ed.), Enzyklopädie der Psychologie: Psychologie der Emotion (Vol. Bd. IV, pp. 115–213). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geppert, U., & Küster, U. (1983). The emergence of “wanting to do it oneself”. A precursor of achievement motivation. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 6, 355–370.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G. (2000). Adaptive thinking: Rationality in the real world. London, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G., Hertwig, R., & Pachur, T. (2011). Heuristics: The foundation of adaptive behavior. New York, NY: Oxfort University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodenough, F. L. (1931). Anger in young children. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottfredson, L. S. (1981). Circumscription and compromise: A developmental theory of occupational aspirations. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 28, 545–579.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graf, F., Lamm, B., Goertz, C., Kolling, T., Freitag, C., Spangler, S., et al. (2012). Infant contingency learning in different cultural contexts. Infant and Child Development, 21, 458–473.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gurack, E. (1978). Die Entwicklung des Fähigkeitskonzepts im Vorschulalter. In Unveröffentlichte Diplomarbeit. Bochum, Germany: RUB, Psychologisches Institut.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halisch, F., & Geppert, U. (2001a). Motives, personal goals, and life satisfaction in old age: First results from the Munich Twin Study (GOLD). In A. Efklides, J. Kuhl, & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Trends and prospects in motivation research (pp. 389–409). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halisch, F., & Geppert, U. (2001b). Genetic vs. environmental determinants of traits, motives, self-referential cognitions, and volitional control in old age: First results from the Munich Twin Study (GOLD). In A. Efklides, J. Kuhl, & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Trends and prospects in motivation research (pp. 359–397). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halisch, C., & Halisch, F. (1980). Kognitive Voraussetzungen frühkindlicher Selbstbewertung-sreaktionen nach Erfolg und Misserfolg. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 12, 193–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Pintrich, P. R., Elliot, A. J., & Thrash, T. M. (2002). Revision of achievement goal theory: Necessary and illuminating. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 638–645.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Tauer, J. M., & Elliot, A. (2002). Predicting success in college: A longitudinal study of achievement goals and ability measure as predictors of interest and performance from freshman year through graduation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 562–575.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harackiewicz, J. M., & Elliot, A. (1993). Achievement goals and intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 904–915.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., & Elliot, A. (1998). Rethinking achievement goals: When are they adaptive for college students and why? Educational Psychologist, 33, 1–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harlow, H. F. (1953). Motivation as a factor in the acquisition of new responses. Current theory and research in motivation: A symposium (pp. 24–49). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harlow, H. F., & Harlow, M. H. (1966). Learning to love. American Scientist, 54, 244–272.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harlow, H. F., & Zimmermann, R. (1959). Affectional responses in the infant monkey. Science, 130, 421–432.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harter, S. (1974). Pleasure derived from cognitive challenge and mastery. Child Development, 45, 661–669.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harter, S. (1978). Effectance motivation reconsidered. Human Development, 21, 34–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayashi, T., & Yamauchi, K. (1964). The relation of children’s need for achievement to their parents’ home discipline in regard to independence and mastery. Bulletin of Kyoto Gakugei University, A25, 31–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, H. (1974). Motivationsanalysen. Berlin, Germany: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, H. (1977). Achievement motivation and its constructs. In A cognitive model. Motivation and Emotion (Vol. 4, 1st ed., pp. 283–329). New York, NY: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, H. (1978). Selbstbewertung nach erwartungswidrigem Leistungsverlauf: Einfluß von Motiv, Kausalattribution und Zielsetzung. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 10, 191–216.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, H. (1980). Motivation und Handeln. Heidelberg, Germany: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, H. (1982). The development of achievement motivation. In W. W. Hartup (Ed.), Review of child development research (pp. 600–668). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, H. (1983). Entwicklungsschritte in der Kausalattribution von Handlungsergebnissen. Kindliche Erklärungsmuster: Entwicklungspsychologische Beiträge zur Attributionsforschung, 1, 49–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, H. (1984). Attributionsmuster für Leistungsergebnisse: Individuelle Unterschiede, mögliche Arten und deren Genese. In F. E. Weinert & R. H. Kluwe (Eds.), Metakognition, Motivation und Lernen (pp. 133–164). Stuttgart, Germany: Kohlhammer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, J. (1987a). Balancing for weaknesses and challenging developmental potential: A longitudinal study of mother-infant dyads in apprenticeship interactions. Developmental Psychology, 23, 762–770.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, J. (1987b). How do mothers know? Infants’ chronological age or infants performance as determinants of adaptation in maternal instructions? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 43, 212–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, J. (1988). Becoming aware of ones competence in the second year: Developmental progression within the mother-child dyad. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 11, 305–326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, J. (1989). Normatives Entwicklungswissen als Bezugsrahmen zur (Re)Konstruktion der eigenen Biographie. In P. Alheit & E. Hörning (Eds.), Biographisches Wissen: Beiträge zu einer Theorie lebensgeschichtlicher Erfahrung (pp. 202–282). Frankfurt, Germany: Campus.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, J. (1993). The development of mastery and its perception within caretaker-child dyads. In D. Messer (Ed.), Mastery motivation in early childhood: Development, measurement and social processes (pp. 55–79). New York, NY: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, J. (1999). Developmental regulation in adulthood: Age-normative and sociostructural constraints as adaptive challenges. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, J. (2000a). Evolutionary perspectives on human motivation. American Behavioral Scientist, 43, 1015–1029.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, J. (2000b). Developmental regulation across the life span: An action-phase model of engagement and disengagement with developmental goals. In J. Heckhausen (Ed.), Motivational psychology of human development. Developing motivation and motivating development (pp. 213–231). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, J. (2005). Competence and motivation in adulthood and old age: Making the most of changing capacities and resources. In A. Elliot & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 240–256). New York, NY: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, J., & Dweck, C. S. (1998). Life span perspectives on motivation and control. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, H., & Meyer, W. U. (1972). Selbständigkeitserziehung und Leistungsmotiv. Forschungsgemeinschaft, 4, 10–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, H., & Roelofsen, I. (1962). Anfänge und Entwicklung der Leistungsmotivation: (I) Im Wetteifer des Kleinkindes. Psychologische Forschung, 26, 313–397.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, J., & Schulz, R. (1995). A life-span theory of control. Psychological Review, 102, 284–304.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, J., & Schulz, R. (1999). The primacy of primary control is a human universal: A reply to Goulds critique of the life-span theory of control. Psychological Review, 106, 605–609.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, H., & Wagner, I. (1965). Anfänge der Entwicklung der Leistungsmotivation: (II) In der Zielsetzung des Kleinkindes. Zur Genese des Anspruchsniveaus. Psychologische Forschung, 28, 179–245.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, J., Wrosch, C., & Schulz, R. (2010). A motivational theory of life-span development. Psychological Review, 117, 32–60.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1999). Is there a universal need for positive self-regard. Psychological Review, 106, 766–794.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hetzer, H. (1931). Kind und Schaffen. Experimente über konstruktive Betätigung im Kleinkindalter. Jena, Germany: Fischer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heyman, D., Dweck, C. S., & Cain, L. (1992). Young children’s vulnerability to self-blame and helplessness. Child Development, 63, 391–403.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heyman, D., Gee, C. L., & Giles, J. W. (2003). Preschool children’s reasoning about ability. Child Development, 74, 516–534.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T., & Silberman, I. (1998). Development of regulatory focus: Promotion and prevention as ways of living. In J. Heckhausen & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Motivation and self regulation across the life span (pp. 78–113). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hofer, J., Busch, H., Bond, M. H., Kärtner, J., Kiessling, F., & Law, R. (2010). Is self-determined functioning a universal prerequisite for motive-goal congruence? Examining the domain of achievement in three cultures. Journal of Personality, 78, 747–779.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hokoda, A., & Fincham, F. D. (1995). Origins of children’s helpless and mastery achievement patterns in the family. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 375–385.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holodynski, M., Seeger, D., Kortas-Hartmann, P., & Wörmann, V. (2013). Placing emotion regulation in a developmental framework of self-regulation. In K. C. Barrett, N. A. Fox, G. A. Morgan, D. J. Fodler, & L. A. Daunhauer (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulatory processes in development: New directions and international perspectives (pp. 27–59). New York, NY: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hulleman, C. S., Schrager, S. M., Bodmann, S. M., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2010). A meta-analytic review of achievement goal measures: Different labels for the same constructs of different constructs with similar labels? Psychological Bulletin, 136, 422–449.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jagacinski, C. M., & Nicholls, J. G. (1987). Competence and affect in task involvement and ego involvement: The impact of social comparison information. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 107–114.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jagacinski, C. M., & Nicholls, J. G. (1990). Reducing effort to protect perceived ability: They’d do it but I wouldn’t. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 15–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Janos, O., & Papoušek, H. (1977). Acquestion of appetition and palpebral conditioned reflexes by the same infants. Early Human Development, 1, 91–97.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jennings, K. D. (1991). Early development of master motivation and its relation to the self-concept. In M. Bullock (Ed.), The development of intentional action: Cognitive, motivational and interactive processes. Contributions to human development (pp. 1–13). Basel, Switzerland: Karger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kagan, S. (1981). Ecology and the acculturation of cognitive and social styles among Mexican American Children. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 3, 111–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karabenick, J. D., & Heller, K. A. (1976). A developmental study of effort and ability attributions. Developmental Psychology, 12, 559–560.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karasawa, M., Little, T. D., Miyashita, T., & Mashima, M. (1997). Japanese children’s action-control beliefs about school performance. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 20, 405–423.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaye, K. (1977). Toward the origin of dialogue. In H. R. Schaffer (Ed.), Studies in mother-infant interaction (pp. 89–117). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keller, H. (2000). Human parent-child relationships from an evolutionary perspective. American Behavioral Scientist, 43, 957–969.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keller, H. (2012). Autonomy and relatedness revisited: Cultural manifestations of universal human needs. Child Development Perspectives, 6, 12–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keller, H. (2016). Psychological autonomy and hierarchical relatedness as organizers of developmental pathways. Philisophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371, 1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keller, H., Lohaus, A., Völker, S., Cappenberg, M., & Chasiotis, A. (1999). Temporal contingency as a measure of interactional quality. Child Development, 70, 474–485.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keller, H., Lohaus, A., Völker, S., Keller, H., Elben, C., & Ball, J. (2003). Warmth and contingency and their relationship to maternal attitudes toward parenting. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 164, 275–292.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, H. H. (1972). Causal schemata and the attribution process. New York, NY: General Learning.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, H. H. (1973). The process of causal attribution. American Psychologist, 28, 107–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kemmler, L. (1957). Untersuchungen über den frühkindlichen Trotz. Psychologische Forschung, 25, 279–338.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koestner, R., Zuckerman, M., & Koestner, R. (1987). Praise, involvement and intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 383–390.

    Google Scholar 

  • Köller, O., Trautwein, U., Lüdtge, O., & Baumert, J. (2006). Zum Zusammenspiel von schulischer Leistung, Selbstkonzept und Interesse in der gymnasialen Oberstufe. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 20, 27–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krapp, A. (1999). Intrinsische Lernmotivation und Interesse. Forschungsansätze und konzeptuelle Überlegungen. Zeitschrift für Pädagogik, 45, 387–406.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krapp, A., Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. (1992). Interest, learning and development. In K. A. Renninger & S. Hidi (Eds.), The role of interest and development (pp. 3–25). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krüger, H. (1978). Anfänge der Entwicklung des Anstrengungskonzepts im Kindergartenalter. Bochum, Germany: Unveröffentlichte Diplomarbeit, RUB, Psychologisches Institut.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (2000). A theory of self-development: Affective fixation and the STAR Model of personality disorders and related styles. In J. Heckhausen (Ed.), Motivational psychology of human development: Developing motivation and motivating development (pp. 187–211). New York, NY: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J., & Völker, S. (1998). Entwicklung und Persönlichkeit. In H. Keller (Ed.), Lehrbuch der Entwicklungspsychologie (pp. 207–240). Bern, Switzerland: Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kun, A. (1977). Evidence for preschoolers’ understanding of causal direction in extended causal sequences. Child Development, 49, 218–222.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kun, A., &Weiner, B. (1973). Necessary versus sufficient causal schemata for success and failure. Journal of Research in Personality, 7, 197–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kun, A., Parsons, J. E., & Ruble, D. (1974). The development of integration processes using ability and effort information to predict outcome. Developmental Psychology, 10, 721–732.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, F. R., & Heckhausen, J. (2002). Stabilisierung und Kontinuität der Persönlichkeit im Lebensverlauf. Enzyklopädie der Psychologie, Serie Entwicklungspsychologie. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, M., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1979). Toward a theory of social cognition: The development of the self. Social interaction and communication during infancy. In New directions for child development (pp. 1–20). New York, NY: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, M., & Goldberg, S. (1969). Perceptual-cognitive development in infancy: A generalized expectancy model as a function of the mother-infant interaction. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 15, 81–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, M., & Ramsay, D. (2002). Cortisol response to embarrassment and shame. Child Development, 73, 1034–1045.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Linnenbrink, E. A., & Pintrich, P. R. (2003). The role of self-efficacy beliefs in student engagement and learning in the classroom. Reading and Writing Quarterly: Overcoming Learning Difficulties, 19, 119–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Little, T. D. (1998). Self-regulation and school performance: Is there optimal level of action-control? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 70, 54–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Little, T. D., & Lopez, D. (1997). Regularities in the development of children’s causality beliefs about school performance across six sociocultural contexts. Developmental Psychology, 33, 165–175.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Little, T. D., Lopez, D., Oettingen, G., & Baltes, P. (2001). A comparative-longitudinal study of action-control beliefs and school performance: On the role of context. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25, 237–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Little, T. D., Oettingen, G., Stetsenko, A., & Baltes, P. B. (1995). Children’s action-control beliefs about school performance: How do American children compare with German and Russian children? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 686–700.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Little, T. D., Stetsenko, A., & Maier, H. (1999). Action control beliefs and school performance: A longitudinal study of Moscow children and adolescents. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 23, 799–823.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lopez, D. F., Little, T. D., Oettingen, G., & Baltes, P. B. (1998). Self regulation and school performance: Is there optimal level of action-control? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 70, 54–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lund, B., Rheinberg, F., & Gladasch, U. (2001). Ein Elterntraining zum motivationsförderlichen Erziehungsverhalten in Leistungskontexten. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 15, 130–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lütkenhaus, P. (1984). Pleasure derived from mastery in three-year olds: Its function for persistence and the influence of maternal behavior. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 7, 343–358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lütkenhaus, P., & Bullock, M. (1991). The development of volitional skills. In M. Bullock (Ed.), The development of intentional action: Cognitive, motivational and interactive processes. Contributions to human development (Vol. 22, pp. 14–23). Basel, Switzerland: Karger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lütkenhaus, P., Grossman, K. E., & Grossman, K. (1985). Transactional influences of infants’ orienting ability and maternal cooperation on competition in three-year-old children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 80, 257–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacIver, D. J., Stipek, D. J., & Daniels, D. H. (1991). Explaining within semester changes in student effort in junior high school and senior high school courses. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 201–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacTurk, R. H., & Morgan, G. A. (1995). Mastery motivation: Origins conceptualization and applications, Advances in applied developmental psychology (Vol. 12). Westport, CT: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maehr, M. L., & Kleiber, D. (1981). The graying of achievement motivation. American Psychologist, 36, 787–793.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, H. W., Abduljabbar, A. S., Morin, A. J. S., Parker, P., Abdelfarrah, F., Nagengast, B., & Abu-Hilal, M. M. (2015). The big-fish-little-pond effect: Generalizability of social comparison processes over two age cohorts from Western, Asian, and Middle-Eastern Islamic countries. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107, 258–271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, H. W., & Hau, K. T. (2003). Big-fish-little-pond effect on academic self-concept: A crosscultural (26 country) test of the negative effects of academically selective schools. American Psychologist, 58, 364–376.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, H. W., Kong, C.-K., & Hau, K.-T. (2000). Longitudinal multilevel models of the big-fish-little-pond effect on academic self-concept: Counterbalancing contrast and reflected-glory effects in Hong Kong schools. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 337–349.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, H. W., Kuyper, H., Seaton, M., Parker, P., Morin, A. J. S., Möller, J., & Abdeljabbar, A. S. (2014). Dimensional comparison theory: An extension of the internal/external frame of reference effect on academic self-concept formation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 39, 326–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, H. W., Trautwein, U., Lüdtke, O., & Köller, O. (2008). Social comparison and big-fish-little-pond effects on self-concept and other self-belief constructs: Role of generalized and specific others. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 510–524.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, D. C. (1985). How motives, skills, and values determine what people do. American Psychologist, 41, 812–825.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, D. C. (1987). Biological aspects of human motivation. In F. Halisch & J. Kuhl (Eds.), Motivation, intention, und volition (pp. 11–19). Berlin, Germany: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, D. C., Koestner, R., & Weinberger, J. (1989). How do self-attributed and implicit motives differ? Psychological Review, 96, 690–702.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, D. C., & Pilon, D. A. (1983). Sources of adult motives in patterns of parent behavior in early childhood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 564–574.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 75–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, W.-U. (1973). Anstrengungsintention in Abhängigkeit von Begabungseinschätzung und Aufgabenschwierigkeit. Archiv für Psychologie, 125, 245–262.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, A. T. (1985). A developmental study of the cognitive basis of performance impairment after failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 529–538.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, A. T. (1987). Changes in self academic self concept in early school years: The role of conceptions of ability. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 2, 551–558.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, A. T., & Hom, H. (1990). Influence of extrinsic and ego incentive value on persistence after failure and continuing motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 539–545.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex frontal lobe tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moffitt, T. E., Arsenault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H., et al. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108, 2693–2698.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Moller, A. C., & Elliot, A. J. (2006). The 2×2 achievement goal framework: An overview of empirical research. In A. V. (Ed.), Focus on educational psychology (pp. 307–326). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Möller, J., & Marsh, H. W. (2013). Dimensional comparison theory. Psychological Review, 120, 544–560.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Murayama, K., & Elliot, A. J. (2009). The joint influence of personal achievement goals and classroom goal structures on achievement-relevant outcomes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 432–447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neuenschwander, R., Röthlisberger, M., Cimeli, P., & Roebers, C. M. (2012). How do different aspects of self-regulation predict successful adaptation to school? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113, 353–371.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls, J. G. (1975). Causal attributions and other achievement-related cognitions: Effects of task outcome, attainment value, and sex. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 379–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls, J. G. (1976). Effort is virtuous, but it’s better to have ability: Evaluative responses to perceptions of effort and ability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10, 306–315.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls, J. G. (1978). The development of the concepts of effort and ability, perception of own attainment, and the understanding that difficult tasks require more than ability. Child Development, 49, 800–814.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls, J. G. (1985). Development and its discontents: The differentiation of the concept of ability. In J. G. Nicholls (Ed.), The development of achievement motivation (pp. 185–218). Greenwich, CT: JAI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls, J. G., & Miller, A. T. (1983). The differentiation of the concepts of difficulty and ability. Child Development, 54, 951–959.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pajares, F. (1996). Self-efficacy beliefs in academic settings. Review of Educational Research, 66, 543–578.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papoušek, H. (1967). Experimental studies of appetitional behavior in human newborns and infants. In H. W. Stevenson, E. H. Hess, & H. L. Rheingold (Eds.), Early behavior: Comparative developmental approaches (pp. 249–277). New York, NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papoušek, H., & Papoušek, M. (1975). Cognitive aspects of preverbal social interaction between human infants and adults. In M. Hofer (Ed.), Parent-infant interaction. Amsterdam, Germany: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papoušek, H., & Papoušek, M. (1987). Intuitive parenting: A dialectic counterpart to the infant’s integrative competence. In J. D. Osofsky (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (2nd ed., pp. 669–720). New York, NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parsons, J. E., & Ruble, D. (1977). The development of achievement-related expectancies. Child Development, 48, 1075–1079.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Passman, R. H., & Erck, T. (1978). Permitting maternal contact through vision alone: Films of mothers for promoting play and locomotion. Developmental Psychology, 14, 512–516.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York, NY: International University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Pintrich, P. R., & De Groot, E. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated learning components of classroom academic performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 33–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pintrich, P. R., & Garcia, T. (1991). Student goal orientation and self-regulation in the college classroom. In M. Maehr & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Advance in motivation and achievement: Goals and self-regulatory processes (Vol. 7, pp. 371–402). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pomerantz, E. M., & Ruble, D. (1997). Distinguishing multiple dimensions of conceptions of ability: Implications for self-evaluations. Child Development, 68, 1165–1180.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ramey, C., & Finkelstein, N. (1978). Contingent stimulation and infant competence. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 3, 89–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reif, M. (1970). Leistungsmotivation in Abhängigkeit vom Erziehungsverhalten der Mutter. Bochum, Germany: Unveröffentlichte Diplomarbeit, RUB, Psychologisches Institut.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rheinberg, F. (1980). Leistungsbewertung und Lernmotivation. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rheinberg, F. (1989). Zweck und Tätigkeit. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rheinberg, F. (2004a). Motivationsdiagnostik. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rheinberg, F. (2004b). Motivational competence and flow-experience. In Paper presented at the 2nd European Conference of Positive Psychology. Verbania, Italy.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rheinberg, F., & Engeser, S. (2011). Motivational competence: The joint effect of implicit and explicit motives on self-regulation and flow experience. In D. Leontiev (Ed.), Motivation, consciousness, and self-regulation (pp. 79–87). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rheinberg, F., & Krug, S. (2005). Motivationsförderung im Schulalltag (3rd ed.). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rheinberg, F. (2006). Motivation (6th ed.). Stuttgart, Germany: Kohlhammer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rheinberg, F., Lührmann, J.-V., & Wagner, H. (1977). Bezugsnormorientierung von Schülern der 5. bis 13. Klassenstufe bei der Leistungsbeurteilung. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogischer Psychologie, 9, 90–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rheinberg, F., Schmalt, H., & Wasser, I. (1978). Ein Lehrerunterschied, der etwas ausmacht. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 10, 3–7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rheingold, H., & Eckerman, C. (1969). The infants free entry into a new environment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 8, 271–283.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rholes, W. S., Blackwell, J., Jordan, A., & Walters, M. (1980). A developmental study of learned helplessness. Developmental Psychology, 16, 616–624.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rholes, W. S., & Ruble, D. (1984). Children’s understanding of dispositional characteristics of others. Child Development, 55, 550–560.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riksen-Walraven, J. M. (1978). Effects of caregiver behavior on habituation rate and self-efficacy in infants. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 1, 105–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roe, A., & Siegelman, M. (1964). The origin of interests. APGA Inquiry Studies, 1, 98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B., & Wertsch, J. (1984). Children’s learning in the “zone of proximal development”. New Directions for Child Development, 23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosen, B. C. (1959). Race, ethnicity, and the achievement syndrome. American Sociological Review, 24, 47–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosen, B. C., & D’Andrade, R. (1959). The psychosocial origins of achievement motivation. Sociometry, 22, 185–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs, 80(609), 1–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rovee, C. K., & Fagan, J. (1976). Extended conditioning and 24 hour retention in infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 21, 1–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ruble, D. N., Boggiano, A., Feldman, N. S., & Loebl, J. H. (1980). A developmental analysis of the role of social comparison in self-evaluation. Developmental Psychology, 16, 105–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruble, D. N., & Martin, C. L. (2002). Conceptualization, measuring and evaluation the developmental course of gender differentiation: Compliments, queries and quandaries. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 67(269), 148–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruble, D. N., Parsons, J. E., & Ross, M. (1976). Self-evaluative responses of children in an achievement setting. Child Development, 48, 1362–1368.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruhland, D., & Feld, S. (1977). The development of achievement motivation in black and white children. Child Development, 48, 1362–1368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sansone, C., Sachau, A., & Weir, C. (1989). Effects of instruction on intrinsic interest: The importance of context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 819–829.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schattke, K., Koestner, R., & Kehr, H. M. (2011). Childhood correlates of adult levels of incongruence between implicit and explicit motives. Motivation and Emotion, 35, 306–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scheffer, D. (2000). Entwicklungsbedingungen impliziter Motive: Bindung, Leistung und Macht. Dissertation, Universität Osnabrück.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmalt, H.-D. (1975). Independence training and various aspects of achievement motivation. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 7, 24–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmalt, H.-D. (1978). Machtmotivation. Psychologische Rundschau, 30, 269–285.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, K., & Unzner, L. (1992). Preschoolers’ attention and emotion in an achievement and an effect game: A longitudinal study. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 37–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schöne, C., Dickhäuser, O., Spinath, B., & Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (2004). Zielorientierung und Bezugsnormorientierung: Zum Zusammenhang zweier Konzepte. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 18, 93–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schulz, R., & Heckhausen, J. (1996). A life-span model of successful aging. American Psychologist, 51, 702–714.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schunk, D. H. (1982). Effects of effort attributional feedback on children’s perceived self-efficacy and achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 548–556.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schurtz, I. M., Pfost, M., Nagengast, B., & Artelt, C. (2014). Impact of social and dimensional comparisons on student’s mathematical and English subject-interest at the beginning of secondary school. Learning and Instruction, 34, 32–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schuster, B., Ruble, D. N., & Weinert, F. E. (1998). Causal inferences and the positivity bias in children: The role of the covariation principle. Child Development, 69, 1577–1596.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sears, R. R., Maccoby, E. E., & Levin, H. (1957). Patterns of child rearing. Evanston, IL: Row Peterson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Senko, C., Hulleman, C. S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2011). Achievement goal theory at the crossroads: Old controversies, current challenges, and new directions. Educational Psychologist, 46, 26–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shi, Y., Chung, J. M., Chen, J. T., Tracy, J. L., Robins, R. W., Chen, X., & Zheng, Y. (2015). Cross-cultural evidence for the two-facet structure of pride. Journal of Research in Personality, 55, 61–74.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S. (2002). Variability and infant development. Infant Behavior & Development, 25, 550–557.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singh, D. (1970). Preference for bar-pressing to obtain reward over freeloading in rats and children. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 73, 320–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, E. A. (1990). Age differences in dimensions of perceived control during middle childhood: Implications for developmental conceptualizations and research. Child Development, 61, 1882–1890.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, E. A. (1996). A guide to constructs of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 549–570.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, E. A., Chapman, M., & Baltes, P. (1988). Control, means-ends, and agency beliefs: A new conceptualization and its measurement during childhood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 117–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, E. A., & Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J. (2007). The development of coping. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 119–144.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, C. P. (Ed.). (1969). The origin and expression of achievement-related motives in children (pp. 102–150). New York, NY: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spangler, G., Bräutigam, I., & Stadler, R. (1984). Handlungsentwicklung in der frühen Kindheit und ihre Abhängigkeit von der kognitiven Entwicklung und der emotionalen Erregbarkeit des Kindes. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 16, 181–193.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spinath, B., & Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (2003). Goal orientation and achievement: The role of ability self-concept and failure perception. Learning and Instruction, 13, 403–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sroufe, L. A. (1977). Weariness of strangers and the study of infant development. Child Development, 48, 731–746.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sroufe, L. A., & Waters, E. (1977). The ontogenesis of smiling and laughter: A perspective on the organization of development in infancy. Psychological Review, 83, 173–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stayton, D. F., Hogan, R., & Ainsworth, M. D. (1971). Infant obedience and maternal behavior: The origins of socialization reconsidered. Child Development, 42, 1057–1069.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stiensmeier-Pelster, J., Balke, S., & Schlangen, B. (1996). Learning goal orientation vs. performance goal orientation as determinants of learning progress. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 28, 169–187.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stipek, D., & Daniels, D. (1990). Children’s use of dispositional attributions in predicting the performance and behavior of classmates. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 11, 13–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stipek, D. J., & Decotis, K. (1988). Children’s understanding of the implications of causal attributions for emotional experiences. Child Development, 59, 1601–1616.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stipek, D., & Hoffman, J. (1980). Development of children’s performance-related judgements. Child Development, 51, 912–914.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stipek, D., & Kowalski, P. (1989). Learned helplessness in task-orienting versus performance-orienting testing conditions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 384–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stipek, D., Recchia, S., & McClintic, S. (1992). Self-evaluation in young children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stipek, D., & Tannatt, L. (1984). Children’s judgments of their own and peers’ academic competence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 75–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Surber, C. F. (1980). The development of reversible operations in judgments of ability, effort and performance. Child Development, 51, 1018–1029.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social-psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193–210.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. (1994). Positive illusions and well-being revisited: Separating fact from fiction. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 21–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Teevan, R. C., & McGhee, P. E. (1972). Childhood development of the fear or failure motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21, 345–348.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, R. (1987). Development of children’s inferences of the emotions of others. Developmental Psychology, 22, 124–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2007a). The psychological structure of pride: A tale of two facets. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 506–525.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2007b). Emerging insights into the nature and function of pride. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 147–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2008). The nonverbal expression of pride: Evidence for cross-cultural recognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 516–530.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tracy, J. L., Robins, R. W., & Lagattuta, K. H. (2005). Can children recognize the pride expression? Emotion, 5, 251–257.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trautwein, U., Lüdtke, O., Marsh, H. W., & Nagy, G. (2009). Within-school social comparison: How students perceive the standing of their class predicts academic self-concept. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 853–866.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1977). Descriptive analyses of infant communicative behavior. In H. R. Schaffer (Ed.), Studies in mother-infant interaction (pp. 227–270). London, UK: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1980). The foundations of intersubjectivity: Development of interpersonal and cooperative understanding in infants. Toronto, Canada: McLeod.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C., & Aitken, K. J. (2001). Infant intersubjectivity: Research, theory, and clinical applications. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 42, 3–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C., & Hubley, P. (1978). Secondary intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding, and acts of meaning in the first year. In Action, gesture, and symbol: The emergence of language. London, UK: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trudewind, C. (1975). Häusliche Umwelt und Motiventwicklung. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trudewind, C. (1982a). The development of achievement motivation and individual differences: Ecological determinants. In W. W. Hartup (Ed.), Review of child development research (Vol. 6, pp. 669–703). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trudewind, C. (1982b). Der ökologische Ansatz in der Erforschung der Leistungsmotivgenese. In L. Vaskowics (Ed.), Umweltbedingungen familialer Sozialisation (pp. 168–203). Stuttgart, Germany: Enke.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trudewind, C. (1987). The role of toy and games in an ecological approach to motive development. In F. Halisch & J. Kuhl (Eds.), Motivation, intention, and volition (pp. 179–199). Berlin, Germany: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Trudewind, C. (2000). Curiosity and anxiety as motivational determinants of cognitive development. In J. Heckhausen (Ed.), Motivational psychology of human development: Developing motivation and motivating development (pp. 15–38). New York, NY: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Trudewind, C., Brünger, T., & Krieger, K. (1986). Parental expectations and the development of achievement motivation. In J. H. van den Bercken, E. DeBruyn, & T. Bergen (Eds.), Achievement and task motivation (pp. 179–200). Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trudewind, C., & Husarek, B. (1979). Mutter-Kind-Interaktion bei der Hausaufgabenbetreuung und die Leistungsmotiventwicklung im Grundschulalter: Analyse einer ökologischen Schlüsselsituation. In H. Walter & R. Oerter (Eds.), Ökologie und Entwicklung (pp. 229–246). Stuttgart, Germany: Klett.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trudewind, C., & Schneider, K. (1994). Individual differences in the development of exploratory behavior: Methodological considerations. In H. Keller, K. Schneider, & B. Henderson (Eds.), Curiosity and exploration (pp. 151–176). Berlin, Germany: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Trudewind, C., Unzner, L., & Schneider, K. (1997). Die Entwicklung der Leistungsmotivation. In H. Keller (Ed.), Handbuch der Kleinkindforschung (pp. 587–622). Bern, Switzerland: Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trudewind, C., & Windel, A. (1991). Elterliche Einflussnahme auf die kindliche Kompetenzentwicklung. Schulleistungsentwicklung und ihre motivationale Vermittlung. In R. Pekrun & H. Fend (Eds.), Schule und Persönlichkeitsentwicklung. Stuttgart, Germany: Enke.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tweer, R. (1976). Das Ökonomieprinzip in der Anstrengungskalkulation: Eine entwicklungspsychologische Untersuchung. Bochum, Germany: Unveröffentlichte Diplomarbeit, RUB, Psychologisches Institut.

    Google Scholar 

  • Twenge, J. M., & Campbell, W. K. (2008). Increases in positive self-views among high school students: Birth-cohort changes in anticipated performance, self-satisfaction, self-liking, and self-competence. Psychological Science, 19, 1082–1086.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Valero, D., Nikitin, J., & Freund, A. M. (2015). The effect of age and time perspective on implicit motives. Motivation and Emotion, 39, 175–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Veroff, J. (1969). Social comparison and the development of achievement motivation. In C. P. Smith (Ed.), Achievement-related motives in children (pp. 46–101). New York, NY: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Veroff, J., Reuman, D., & Feld, S. C. (1984). Motives in American men and women across the adult life span. Developmental Psychology, 20, 1142–1158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vierhaus, M., Lohaus, A., & Ball, J. (2007). Developmental changes in coping: Situational and methodological influences. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 20, 267–282.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotski, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. (1969). Levels of symbolization in adolescent adjustment patterns. International Journal of Symbology, 1, 67–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wasna, M. (1970). Die Entwicklung der Leistungsmotivation. München, Germany: Reinhardt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. S. (1966). The development and generalization of contingency awareness in early infancy: Some hypotheses. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 12, 123–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. S. (1972). Smiling, cooing, and the Game. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 18, 323–339.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. S., & Ramey, C. (1972). Reactions to response contingent stimulation in early infancy. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 18, 219–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weinberger, J., & McClelland, D. C. (1990). Cognitive versus traditional motivational models: Irreconcilable or complementary? In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (2nd ed., pp. 562–597). New York, NY: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B. (1972). Theories of motivation. Chicago: Markham.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B., Kun, A., & Benesh-Weiner, M. (1980). The development of mastery, emotions, and morality from an attributional perspective. Minnesota Symposia on Child Development, 13, 103–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B., & Peter, N. (1973). A cognitive-developmental analysis of achievement and moral judgements. Developmental Psychology, 9, 290–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weisz, J. R. (1983). Can I control it? The pursuit of veridical answers across the life span. Life-span Development and Behavior, 3, 233–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weisz, J. R., Yeates, K. O., Robertson, D., & Beckham, J. C. (1982). Perceived contingency of skill and chance events: A developmental analysis. Developmental Psychology, 18, 898–905.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Welker, W. L. (1956). Some determinants of play and exploration in chimpanzees. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 49, 84–89.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wentzel, K. R. (1989). Adolescent classroom goals, standards for performance and academic achievement: An interactionist perspective. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 131–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wertlieb, D., Weigel, C., & Feldstein, M. (1987). Measuring children’s coping. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 57, 548–560.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence. Psychological Review, 66, 297–333.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S., Yoon, K. S., & Harold, R. D. (1997). Change in children’s competence beliefs and subjective task values across the elementary school years: A 3-year study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 451–469.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winter, D. G. (1973). The power motive. New York: Free Press..

    Google Scholar 

  • Winterbottom, M. R. (1958). The relation of need for achievement to learning experiences in independence and mastery. In J. W. Atkinson (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action and society (pp. 453–478). Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.

    Google Scholar 

  • Witkowski, T., & Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (1998). Performance deficits following failure: Learned helplessness or self-esteem protection? British Journal of Educational Psychology, 37, 59–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, D. J., Bruner, J. S., & Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem solving. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 17, 89–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wrosch, C., & Miller, G. E. (2009). Depressive symptoms can be useful: Self-regulatory and emotional benefits of dysphoric mood in adolescence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 1181–1190.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yarrow, L. J., McQuiston, S., MacTurk, R. H., McCarthy, M. E., Klein, R. P., & Vietze, P. M. (1983). Assessment of mastery motivation during the first year of life. Contemporaneous and cross-age relationships. Developmental Psychology, 19, 159–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yussen, S., & Kane, P. (1985). Children’s conceptions of intelligence. In S. R. Yussen (Ed.), The growth of reflection in children (pp. 207–241). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. D. (2004). The development of conscious control in childhood. Trends in Cognitive Science, 8, 12–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. D., & Carlson, S. M. (2005). Hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence: Development and plasticity. Child Development Perspectives, 6, 354–360.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. D. (2015). Executive function: Reflection, iterative reprocessing, complexity, and the developing brain. Developmental Review, 38, 55–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. D., Anderson, J. E., Richler, J., Wallner-Allen, K., Beaumont, J. L. & Weintraub, S. (2013). NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): Measuring executive function and attention. In P. D. Zelazo & P. J. Bauer (Hrsg.), National Institutes of Health Toolbox – Cognition Battery (NIH Toolbox CB): Validation for children between 3 and 15 years (S. 16–33). Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 78 (4, Serial No. 309).

    Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. D., & Carlson, S. M. (2012). Hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence: Development and plasticity. Child Development Perspectives, 6, 354–360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, A., & Heller, K. A. (2000). Effects of an attribution retraining with female students gifted in physics. Journal of the Education of the Gifted, 23, 217–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, A., & Stöger, H. (2004). Evaluation of an attributional retraining (modeling technique) to reduce gender differences in chemistry instruction. High Ability Studies, 15, 63–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jutta Heckhausen .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Heckhausen, J., Heckhausen, H. (2018). Development of Motivation. In: Heckhausen, J., Heckhausen, H. (eds) Motivation and Action . Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-65094-4_16

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics