Skip to main content

Visualizing Chance: Tackling Conditional Probability Misconceptions

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Topics and Trends in Current Statistics Education Research

Part of the book series: ICME-13 Monographs ((ICME13Mo))

Abstract

Probabilistic reasoning is essential for operating sensibly and optimally in the 21st century. However, research suggests that students have many difficulties in understanding conditional probabilities and that Bayesian-type problems are replete with misconceptions such as the base rate fallacy and confusion of the inverse. Using a dynamic pachinkogram, a visual representation of the traditional probability tree , we explore six undergraduate probability students’ reasoning processes as they interact with this tool. Initial findings suggest that in simulating a screening situation, the ability to vary the branch widths of the pachinkogram may have the potential to convey the impact of the base rate. Furthermore, we conjecture that the representation afforded by the pachinkogram may help to clarify the distinction between probabilities with inverted conditions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    The word pachinkogram originates from the Japanese pachinko machine which resembles a vertical pinball machine.

  2. 2.

    Note +ve denotes a positive test result. Therefore \(P({\text{Diabetic}}\,|\,{{ {+}\text{ve})}}\) is the probability of having diabetes, given that a person has a positive test result and \(P({{{+}\text{ve}}}\,|\,{\text{Diabetic}})\) is the probability of having a positive test result, given that a person is diabetic.

  3. 3.

    Students were provided with a table of data from Coppell et al. (2013) from which they could determine the base rate for the subgroup of the population referred to in the questions.

References

  • Arcavi, A. (2003). The role of visual representations in the learning of mathematics. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 52, 215–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arnold, P., Pfannkuch, M., Wild, C., Regan, M., & Budgett, S. (2011). Enhancing students’ inferential reasoning: From hands-on to “movies”. Journal of Statistics Education, 19(2), 1–32. Retrieved from http://www.amstat.org/publications/jse/v19n2/pfannkuch.pdf.

  • Bakker, A. (2004). Reasoning about shape as a pattern in variability. Statistics Education Research Journal, 3(2), 64–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Hillel, M. (1980). The base rate fallacy in probability judgments. Acta Psychologica, 44, 211–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Batanero, C., Chernoff, E., Engel, J., Lee, H., & Sánchez, E. (2016). Research on teaching and learning probability. In Proceedings of Topic Study Group 14 at the 13th International Conference on Mathematics Education (ICME), Hamburg, Germany (pp. 1–33). https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-31625-3_1.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bea, W. (1995). Stochastisches denken [Statistical reasoning]. Frankfurt am Main, Germany: Peter Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biehler, R. (1991). Computers in probability education. In R. Kapadia & M. Borovnick (Eds.), Chance encounters: Probability in education (pp. 169–211). Boston, MA: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Binder, K., Krauss, S., & Bruckmaier, G. (2015). Effects of visualizing statistical information—An empirical study on tree diagrams and 2 × 2 tables. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(1186). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01186.

  • Böcherer-Linder, K., Eichler, A., & Vogel, M. (2016). The impact of visualization on understanding conditional probabilities. In Proceedings of the 13th International Congress on Mathematical Education,Hamburg (pp. 1–4). Retrieved from http://iase-web.org/documents/papers/icme13/ICME13_S1_Boechererlinder.pdf.

  • Borovnick, M. (2011). Strengthening the role of probability within statistics curricula. In C. Batanero, G. Burrill, & C. Reading (Eds.), Teaching statistics in school mathematics—Challenges for teaching and teacher education: A joint ICMI/IASE study: The 18th ICMI study (pp. 71–83). New York, NY: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brase, G. L. (2009). Pictorial representations in statistical reasoning. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 369–381. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brase, G. L. (2014). The power of representation and interpretation: Doubling statistical reasoning performance with icons and frequentist interpretation of ambiguous numbers. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26(1), 81–97. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.861840.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 3(2), 77–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Budgett, S., Pfannkuch, M., Regan, M., & Wild, C. J. (2013). Dynamic visualizations and the randomization test. Technology Innovations in Statistics Education, 7(2), 1–21. Retrieved from http://escholarship.org/uc/item/9dg6h7wb.

  • Chernoff, E. J., & Sriraman, B. (Eds.). (2014). Probabilistic thinking: Presenting plural perspectives. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-7155-0.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, J., & Paivio, A. (1991). Dual coding theory and education. Educational Psychology Review, 3(3), 149–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cobb, G. (2007). One possible frame for thinking about experiential learning. International Statistical Review, 75(3), 336–347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coppell, K. J., Mann, J. I., Williams, S. M., Jo, E., Drury, P. L., Miller, J., et al. (2013). Prevalence of diagnosed and undiagnosed diabetes and prediabetes in New Zealand: Findings from the 2008:2009 Adult Nutrition Survey. The New Zealand Medical Journal, 126(1370), 23–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Finger, R., & Bisantz, A. M. (2002). Utilizing graphical formats to convey uncertainty in a decision-making task. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science, 3(1), 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1080/14639220110110324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garcia-Retamero, R., & Hoffrage, U. (2013). Visual representation of statistical information improves diagnostic inferences in doctors and patients. Social Science and Medicine, 83, 27–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garfield, J., delMas, R., & Zieffler, A. (2012). Developing statistical modelers and thinkers in an introductory, tertiary-level statistics course. ZDM—International Journal on Mathematics Education, 44(7), 883–898.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G. (2014). Risk savvy: How to make good decisions. New York, NY: Viking.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G., & Hoffrage, U. (1995). How to improve Bayesian reasoning without instruction: Frequency formats. Psychological Bulletin, 102, 684–704.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G., Gaissmaier, W., Kurz-Milcke, E., Schwartz, L. M., & Woloshin, S. (2007). Helping doctors and patients make sense of health statistics. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 8, 53–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G., Hoffrage, U., & Ehert, A. (1998). AIDS counseling for low-risk clients. AIDS Care, 10, 197–211. https://doi.org/10.1080/09540129850124451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greer, B., & Mukhopadhyay, S. (2005). Teaching and learning the mathematization of uncertainty: Historical, cultural, social and political contexts. In G. A. Jones (Ed.), Exploring probability in school: Challenges for teaching and learning (pp. 297–324). New York, NY: Kluwer/Springer Academic Publishers.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, T. L., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2006). Optimal predictions in everyday cognition. Psychological Science, 17, 767–773. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01780.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffrage, U., Hafenbrädl, S., & Bouquet, C. (2015). Natural frequencies facilitate diagnostic inferences of managers. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(642), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00642.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York, NY: Allen Lane.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1973). On the psychology of prediction. Psychological Review, 80, 237–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koehler, J. J. (1996). The base rate fallacy reconsidered: Descriptive, normative and methodological challenges. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19, 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00041157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Konold, C., & Kazak, S. (2008). Reconnecting data and chance. Technology Innovations in Statistics Education, 2(1). Retrieved from http://escholarship.org/uc/item/38p7c94v.

  • Lane, D. M., & Peres, S. C. (2006). Interactive simulations in the teaching of statistics: Promise and pitfalls. In B. Phillips (Ed.), Proceedings of the Seventh International Conference on Teaching Statistics, Cape Town, South Africa. Voorburg, The Netherlands: International Statistical Institute.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lefevre, R. J., Pfautz, J., & Jones, K. (2005). Weather forecast uncertainty management and display. In Proceedings of the 21st International Conference on Interactive Information Processing Systems (UPS) for Meteorology, Oceanography, and Hydrology, San Diego, CA. Retrieved from https://ams.confex.com/ams/pdfpapers/82400.pdf.

  • Makar, K., & Confrey, J. (2005). “Variation-Talk”: Articulating meaning in statistics. Statistics Education Research Journal, 4(1), 27–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandel, D. R. (2015). Instruction in information structuring improves Bayesian judgment in intelligence analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(387), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E. (2009). Multimedia learning. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E. (2010). Unique contributions of eye-tracking research to the study of learning graphics. Learning and Instruction, 20, 167–171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2009.02.012.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, D. (1997). Probability and statistics in the core curriculum. In J. Dossey (Ed.), Confronting the core curriculum (pp. 93–98). Washington, DC: Mathematical Association of America.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nance, D. A., & Morris, S. B. (2005). Juror understanding of DNA evidence: An empirical assessment of presentation formats for trace evidence with a relatively small random-match probability. Journal of Legal Studies, 34, 395–444. https://doi.org/10.1086/428020.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, D. L., Hood, M., & Neumann, M. M. (2013). Using real-life data when teaching statistics: Student perceptions of this strategy in an introductory statistics course. Statistics Education Research Journal, 12(2), 59–70. Retrieved from https://iase-web.org/documents/SERJ/SERJ12(2)_Neumann.pdf.

  • Paling, J. (2003). Strategies to help patients understand risks. British Medical Journal, 327, 745–748. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.327.7417.745.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pfannkuch, M., & Budgett, S. (2016a). Reasoning from an Eikosogram: An exploratory study. International Journal of Research in Undergraduate Mathematics Education, 1–28. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40753-016-0043-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pfannkuch, M., & Budgett, S. (2016b). Markov processes: Exploring the use of dynamic visualizations to enhance student understanding. Journal of Statistics Education, 24(2), 63–73. https://doi.org/10.1080/10691898.2016.1207404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pfannkuch, M., Budgett, S., & Arnold, P. (2015). Experiment-to-causation inference: Understanding causality in a probabilistic setting. In A. Zieffler & E. Fry (Eds.), Reasoning about uncertainty: Learning and teaching informal inferential reasoning (pp. 95–127). Minneapolis, MN: Catalyst Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pfannkuch, M., Budgett, S., Fewster, R., Fitch, M., Pattenwise, S., Wild, C., et al. (2016). Probability modelling and thinking: What can we learn from practice? Statistics Education Research Journal, 11–37. Retrieved from http://iase-web.org/documents/SERJ/SERJ15(2)_Pfannkuch.pdf.

  • Pfannkuch, M., Seber, G. A., & Wild, C. J. (2002). Probability with less pain. Teaching Statistics, 24(1), 24–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pouget, A., Beck, J. M., Ma, W. J., & Latham, P. E. (2013). Probabilistic brains: Knowns and unknowns. Nature Neuroscience, 16, 1170–1178. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3495.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sacristan, A., Calder, N., Rojano, T., Santos-Trigo, M., Friedlander, A., & Meissner, H. (2010). The influence and shaping of digital technologies on the learning—and learning trajectories—of mathematical concepts. In C. Hoyles, & J. Lagrange (Eds.), Mathematics education and technology—Rethinking the terrain: The 17th ICMI Study (pp. 179–226). New York, NY: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schoenfeld, A. (2007). Method. In F. Lester (Ed.), Second handbook of research on the teaching and learning of mathematics (pp. 96–107). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sedlmeier, P., & Gigerenzer, G. (2001). Teaching Bayesian reasoning in less than two hours. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 3, 380–400. https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-3445.130.3.380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaughnessy, M. (2007). Research on statistics learning and reasoning. In F. Lester (Ed.), Second handbook of research on the teaching and learning of mathematics (Vol. 2, pp. 957–1009). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sirota, M., VallĂ©e-Tourangeau, G., VallĂ©e-Tourangeau, F., & Juanchich, M. (2015). On Bayesian problem-solving: Helping Bayesians solve simple Bayesian word problems. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(1141), 1–4. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sloman, S. A., Over, D. E., Slovak, L., & Stibel, J. M. (2003). Frequency illusions and other fallacies. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 91, 296–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spiegelhalter, D. J. (n.d.). Screening tests. Retrieved from Understanding Uncertainty: https://understandinguncertainty.org/screening.

  • Sturm, A., & Eichler, A. (2014). Students’ beliefs about the benefit of statistical knowledge when perceiving information through daily media. In K. Makar, B. de Sousa, & R. Gould (Eds.), Proceedings of the Ninth International Conference on Teaching Statistics (ICOTS9), Flagstaff, Arizona, USA. Voorburg, The Netherlands: International Statistical Institute.

    Google Scholar 

  • TĂ©glás, E., Vul, E., Girotto, V., Gonzalez, M., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Bonatti, L. L. (2011). Pure reasoning in 12-month-old infants as probabilistic inference. Science, 1054–1059. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1196404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Teigen, K. H., & Keren, G. (2007). Waiting for the bus: When base-rates refuse to be neglected. Cognition, 103, 337–357. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.03.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, M. O. (2008). Conceptual representations and versatile mathematical thinking. In Proceedings of the Tenth International Congress in Mathematics Education, Copenhagen, Denmark (pp. 1–18).

    Google Scholar 

  • Villejoubert, G., & Mandel, D. R. (2002). The inverse fallacy: An account of deviations from Bayes theorem and the additivity principle. Memory & Cognition, 30, 171–178. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ware, C. (2008). Visual thinking for design. Burlington, MA: Morgan Kaufmann Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. M., & Callingham, R. (2014). Two-way tables: Issues at the heart of statistics and probability for students and teachers. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 16(4), 254–284. https://doi.org/10.1080/10986065.2014.953019.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, C. R. (1995). Information seeking on Bayesian conditional probability problems: A fuzzy-trace theory. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 8, 85–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zikmund-Fisher, B. J., Witteman, H. O., Dickson, M., Fuhrel-Forbis, A., Khan, V. C., Exe, N. L., et al. (2014). Blocks, ovals, or people? Icon type affects risk perceptions and recall of pictographs. Medical Decision Making, 34, 443–453. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989X13511706.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work is supported by a grant from the Teaching and Learning Research Initiative (http://www.tlri.org.nz/).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stephanie Budgett .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Appendix: Some of the Diabetes Task Questions

Appendix: Some of the Diabetes Task Questions

A new housing development has been built in your neighbourhood. In order to service the needs of this new community, a new health clinic has opened. As part of the health clinic’s enrolment procedure, new patients are required to undergo health check-ups which include, among other things, a series of blood tests. One such test is designed to measure the amount of glucose in an individual’s blood. This measurement is recorded after the individual fasts (abstains from eating) for a prescribed period of time. Fasting blood glucose levels in excess of 6.5 mmol/L are deemed to be indicative of diabetes. This threshold of 6.5 mmol/L works most of the time with about 94% of people who have diabetes being correctly classified as diabetics and about 98% of those not having diabetes being correctly classified as non-diabetics.

The prevalence of diabetes in the NZ population is about 7% (i.e. approximately 7% of the NZ population are estimated to have diabetes).

figure a

Graph above adapted from Pfannkuch et al. (2002, p. 28)

  • Question 1

    1. (a)

      As part of enrolment in this health clinic, an individual has a fasting blood test. He/she is told that his/her blood glucose level is higher than 6.5 mmol/L. What are the chances that he/she has diabetes? Provide an intuitive answer.

    2. (b)

      Now use the software tool to answer Question 1 (a).

    3. (c)

      Reflecting on your answer to (b), how does this compare with your answer to (a)?

  • Question 2

    1. (a)

      As part of his enrolment in this health clinic, a male aged between 65 and 74 has a fasting blood test.Footnote 3 He is told that his blood glucose level is higher than 6.5 mmol/L. What are the chances that he has diabetes? Provide an intuitive answer.

    2. (b)

      Now use the software tool to answer Question 2 (a).

    3. (c)

      Reflecting on your answer to (b), how does this compare with your answer to (a)?

  • Question 3

    1. (a)

      As part of their enrolment in this health clinic, a person of Pacific ethnicity and aged over 75 has a fasting blood test. He/she is told that his/her blood glucose level is higher than 6.5 mmol/L. What are the chances that he/she has diabetes? Provide an intuitive answer.

    2. (b)

      Now use the software tool to answer Question 3 (a).

    3. (c)

      Reflecting on your answer to (b), how does this compare with your answer to (a)?

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2019 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Budgett, S., Pfannkuch, M. (2019). Visualizing Chance: Tackling Conditional Probability Misconceptions. In: Burrill, G., Ben-Zvi, D. (eds) Topics and Trends in Current Statistics Education Research. ICME-13 Monographs. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-03472-6_1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-03472-6_1

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-030-03471-9

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-030-03472-6

  • eBook Packages: EducationEducation (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics