Skip to main content

Toward an Action-Based Theory of Infant Development

  • Chapter
Action in Social Context

Part of the book series: Perspectives in Developmental Psychology ((PDPS))

Abstract

The purpose of this chapter is to explore a theory of infant development grounded in an analysis of action. This account differs from customary theories of early development in several ways. First, it is preeminently a movement-based, “bottom-up” account of behavior in the sensorimotor period. Developmentalists, like other psychologists, have been concerned primarily with the formation of the complex symbolic and affective processes of the “life of the mind” and have paid less attention to the translation of ideas into movement—a “life of the limbs.” Infants, however, are born with much movement and few ideas and, for the first year or so, lack symbolic and verbal mediating mechanisms between their mental state and the expressions of their bodies and limbs. At this stage of the life cycle, then, the link between the developing mind and the developing limbs may be especially direct. We see this formulation in no way competing with theories that focus more directly on mental structures but rather as a complement and supplement to understanding the development of cognition.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Abraham, R. H., Sr Shaw, C. D. (1982). Dynamics—The geometry of behavior. Santa Cruz, CA: Aerial Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alberch, P. (1982). The generative and regulatory roles of development in evolution. In C. Roth (Ed.), Environmental adaptation and evolution, (pp. 19–36 ). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anokhin, P. K. (1964). Systemogenesis as a general regulator of brain development. Progress in Brain Research, 9, 54–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arutyunyan, G. H., Gurfinkel, V. S., & Mirksy, M. L. (1969). Investigation of aiming at a target. Biophysics, 13, 536–538.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. B. (1984). Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother-infant and peer-infant interaction. Child Development, 55, 1278–1289.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E. (1979). The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 21, 205–226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., O’Connell, B., Vaid, J., Sledge, P., & Oakes, L. (1985). Language and hand preference in early development. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Toronto.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, P. (1984). Sudden changes in ontogeny and phylogeny. In G. Greenberg & E. Tobach (Eds.), Behavioral evolution and integrative levels, (pp. 155–166 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein, N. (1967). Co-ordination and regulation of movements. New York: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bertalanffy, L. von. (1986). General system theory. New York: George Braziller.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, L., Lifter, K., & Broughton, J. (1981). What children say and what they know: Exploring the relations between product and process in the development of early words and early concepts. In R. Stark (Ed.), Language behavior in infancy and early childhood, (pp. 301–326 ). New York: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bosma, J. F. (1975). Anatomic and physiologic development in the speech apparatus. In D. B. Tower (Ed.), The Nervous System, Vol. 3: Human Communication and its disorders, (pp. 469–481 ). New York: Raven Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brazelton, T. B. (1973). Neonatal behavioral assessment scale. London: S.I.M.P. and Heinemann Medical Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brent, S. B. (1978). Prigoginé s model for self-organization in nonequilibrium systems: Its relevance for developmental psychology. Human Development, 21 374–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1975). The ontogenesis of speech acts. Journal of Child Language, 2, 1–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, J. E., & Phillips, S. J. (1985). The organization of upright locomotion. Paper presented at the Biannual Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Toronto.

    Google Scholar 

  • Connolly, K., & Elliott, J. (1972). The evolution and ontogeny of hand function. In N. Blurton Jones (Ed.), Ethological studies of child behavior, (pp. 329–384 ). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooke, J. D. (1980). The organization of simple, skilled movements. In G. E. Stelmach and J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in Motor Behavior, (pp. 199–212 ). New York: North-Holland.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Demos, V. (1982). Facial expressions of infants and toddlers: A descriptive analysis. In T. Field & A. Fogel (Eds.), Emotion and early interaction, (pp. 127–160 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., Friessen, W., & Ellsworth, P. (1972). Emotion in the human face. New York: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engberg, I., & Lundberg, A. (1969). An electromyographic analysis of muscular activity in the hindlimb of the cat during unrestrained locomotion. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica, 75, 614–630.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fentress, J. C. (1984). The development of coordination. Journal of Motor Behavior, 16, 99–134.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fogel, A. (1980). The effect of brief separations on two-month-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 3, 315–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fogel, A. (1981). The ontogeny of gestural communication: The first six months. In R. E. Stark (Ed.), Language behavior in infancy and early childhood, (pp. 17–44 ). New York: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fogel, A. (1982a). Affect dynamics in early infancy: Affective tolerance. In T. Field & A. Fogel (Eds.), Emotion and early interaction, (pp. 25–26 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fogel, A. (1982b). Social play, positive affect and coping skills in the first six months of life. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 2, 53–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fogel, A. (1985). Coordinative structures in the development of expressive behavior in early infancy. In G. Zivin (Ed.), The development of expressive behavior: Biology-environment interaction, (pp. 249–267 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fogel, A., & Hannan, T. E. (1985). Manual actions of two-to three-month-old human infants during social interaction. Child Development, 56, 1271–1279.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Forssberg, H. (1985). Ontogeny of human locomotor control. I. Infant stepping, supported locomotion, and transition to independent locomotion. Experimental Brain Research, 57, 480–493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gesell, A. (1945). The embryology of behavior. New York: Harper.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gierer, A. (1981). Generation of biological patterns and form: Some physical, mathematical, and logical aspects. Progress in Biophysics and Molecular Biology, 37, 1–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, B. C. (1982). Development and evolution. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 97, 43–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gould, S. J. (1977). Ontogeny and phylogeny. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greene, P. H. (1972). Problems of organization of motor systems. In R. Rosen & F. Snell (Eds.), Progress in theoretical biology, (pp. 303–338 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grillner, S. (1985). Neurobiological bases of rhythmic motor acts in vertebrates. Science, 228, 143–149.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gustafson, G. E. (1984). Effects of the ability to locomote on infants’ social and exploratory behaviors: An experimental study. Developmental Psychology, 20, 397–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haken, H. (1977). Synergetics: An introduction. Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hannan, T. E. (1981). Infant “pointing” behavior in the first three months of life. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Purdue University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hannan, T. E. (1982). Young infant’s hand and finger movements: An analysis of category reliability. In T. Field and A. Fogel (Eds.), Emotion and early interaction, (pp. 253–266 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heriza, C. (1985). The organization of spontaneous leg movements in premature infants. Paper presented at Bienniel Meeting, Society for Research in Child Development, Toronto.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoyt, D. F., & Taylor, C. R. (1981). Gait and the energetics of locomotion in horses. Nature, 292, 239–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iberall, A. S. (1972). Toward a general science of viable systems. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iberall, A. S., & Soodak, H. (1978). Physical basis for complex systems -Some propositions relating levels of organization. Collecting Phenomena, 3, 9–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katchalsky, A. K., Rowland, V., & Blumenthal, R. (Eds.) (1974). Dynamic patterns of brain cell assemblies. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaye, K. (1982). The mental and social life of babies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaye, K., & Fogel, A. (1980). The temporal structure of face-to-face communication between mothers and infants. Developmental Psychology, 16, 454–464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelso, J. A. S., & Tuller, B. (1984). A dynamical basis for action systems. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), Handbook of cognitive neuroscience, (pp. 321–356 ). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelso, J. A. S., Tuller, B., Bateson, E. V., & Fowler, C. A. (1984). Functionally specific articulatory cooperation adaptation to jaw perturbations during speech: Evidence for coordinative structures. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 812–832.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelso, J. A. S., Holt, K. G., Kugler, P. N., & Turvey, M. T. (1980). On the concept of coordinative structures as dissipative structures: II. Empirical lines of convergence. In G. E. Stelmach & J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior, (pp. 49–70 ). New York: North-Holland

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kelso, J. A. S., Southard, D. I., & Goodman, D. (1979). On the nature of human interlimb coordination. Science, 203, 1029–1031.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kent, R. D. (1981). Articulatory-acoustic perspective on speech development. In R. Stark (Ed.), Language development in infancy and early childhood, (pp. 105–126 ). New York: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kessen, W., Haith, M., & Salapatek, P. (1970). Infancy. In P. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child psychology, ( 3rd ed., pp. 287–445 ). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kienapple, K. G. P. (1983). From recognition to categorization; The development of conceptual knowledge in 7- and 10-month-old infants. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Purdue University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitchener, R. F. (1982). Holism and the organismic model in developmental psychgology. Human Development, 25, 233–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Konner, M. (1977). Maternal care, infant behavior and development among the Kalahari Desert San. In R. B. Lee & I. DeVore (Eds.), Kalahari hunter gatherers, (pp. 218–245 ). Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kugler, P. N., Kelso, J. A. S., & Turvey, M. T. (1980). On the concept of coordinative structures as dissipative structures. I. Theoretical lines of convergence. In G. E. Stelmach & J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior, (pp. 3–47 ). New York; North-Holland.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kugler, P., Kelso, J. A. S., & Turvey, M. T. (1982). On the control and co-ordination of naturally developing systems. In J. A. S. Kelso & J. E. Clark (Eds.), The development of movement control and co-ordination, (pp. 5–78 ). New York: John Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lempert, H., & Kinsbourne, M. (1985). Possible origin of speech in selective orienting. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 62–73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leung, E., & Rheingold, H. (1981). The development of pointing as a social gesture. Developmental Psychology, 17, 215–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindblom, B., & Sundberg, J. (1969). A quantitative model of vowel production and the distinctive features of Swedish vowels. Quarterly Progress Status Report, Speech Transmission Laboratory, Royal Institute of Technology, ( Stockholm, Sweden ), 1, 14–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Masur, E. F. (1983). Gestural development, dual directional signalling, and the transition to words. Journal of Psycho-linguistic Research, 12, 93–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGraw, M. B. (1932). From reflex to muscular control in the assumption of an erect posture and ambulation in the human infant. Child Development, 3, 291–297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGraw, M. B. (1940). Neuromuscular development of the human infant as exemplified in the achievement of erect locomotion. Journal of Pediatrics, 17, 747–771.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McMahon, T. A. (1984). Muscles, reflexes, and locomotion. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, C. M., & Messer, D. J. (1977). Mothers, infants, and pointing; A study of gesture. In H. R. Schaffer (Ed.), Studies in mother-infant interaction, (pp. 325–354 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nashner, L. M. (1981). Analysis of stance posture in humans. In A. L. Towe & E. S. Luschei (Eds.), Handbook of behavioral neurobiology, Vol. 5: Motor coordination, (pp. 527–565 ). New York; Plenum Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Netsell, R. (1981). The acquisition of speech motor control. In R. Stark (Ed.), Language behavior in infancy and early childhood, (pp. 127–156 ). New York: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oster, H. (1978). Facial expression and affect development. In M. Lewis & L. A. Rosenblum (Eds.), The development of affect, (pp. 43–75 ). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Overton, W., Reese, H. (1973). Models of development: Methodological implications. In J. Nesselroade & H. Reese (Eds.), Life-span developmental psychology: Methodological Issues, (pp. 65–86 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pearson, K. G., & Duysens, J. (1976). Function of segmental reflexes in the control of stepping in cockroaches and cats. In R. M. Herman, S. Grillner, P. S. G. Stein, & D. G. Stuart (Eds.), Neural control of locomotion, (pp. 519–537 ). New York; Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peiper, A. (1963). Cerebral function in infancy and childhood. New York: Consultants Bureau.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1971). Biology and knowledge. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Platzman, K. (1983). The ontogeny of pointing in the first ten months of life. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prechtl, H. F. R. (1974). The behavioural states of the newborn infant (a review). Brain Research, 76, 185–212.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prigogine, I. (1980). From being to becoming. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramsay, D. S. (1984). Onset of duplicated syllable babbling and unimanual handedness in infancy: Evidence for developmental change in hemispheric specialization? Developmental Psychology, 20, 64–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, E. S. (1982). An outline of a theory of action systems. Journal of Motor Behavior, 14, 98–134.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reese, H. W., & Overton, W. F. (1970). Models of development and theories of development. In L. R. Goulet & P. B. Baltes (Eds.), Life-span development psychology: Research and theory, (pp. 115–145 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosen, R. (1978). Fundamentals of measurement and representation of natural systems. New York: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruff, H. (1984). Infants’ manipulative exploration of objects: Effects of age and object characteristics. Developmental Psychology, 20, 9–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sameroff, A. J. (1983). Developmental systems: Contexts and evolution. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology. History, theory, and methods, (4th ed., Vol. I, pp. 237294 ). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, R. A. (1982). Motor control and learning: A behavioral emphasis. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, D. C., & Schmidt, R. A. (1982). The schema theory: Recent evidence and developmental implications. In J. A. S. Kelso and J. E. Clark (Eds.), The development of movement control and co-ordination, (pp. 113–150 ). New York: John Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, D. C., Zernicke, R. F., Gregor, R. J., & Diestel, J. D. (1981). Evidence for generalized motor program using gait pattern analysis. Journal of Motor Behavior, 13, 33–47.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shik, M. L., & Orlovsky, G. N. (1976). Neurophysiology of locomotor automatism. Physiological Reviews, 56, 465–501.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Soll, D. R. (1979). Timers in developing systems. Science, 203, 841–849.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sroufe, L. A. (1979). Socioemotional development. In J. Osofsky (Ed.), Handbook of infant development, (pp. 462–518 ). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Statham, L., & Murray, M. P. (1971). Early walking patterns of normal children. Clinical Orthopaedics, 79, 8–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Super, C. M. (1980). Behavioral development in infancy. In R. H. Monroe, R. L. Monroe, & B. B. Whiting (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural human development, (pp. 181–270 ). New York: Garland STPM.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutherland, D. H., Olshen, R., Cooper, L., & Woo, S. L.-Y. (1980). The development of mature gait. The Journal of Bone and Joint Surgery, 62, 336–353.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E. (1979). Rhythmical stereotypies in normal human infants. Animal Behaviour, 27, 699–715.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E. (1981a). Kicking, rocking, and waving: Contextual analysis of rhythmical stereotypies in normal human infants. Animal Behaviour, 29, 3–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E. (1981b). Rhythmical behavior in infancy: An ethological perspective. Developmental Psychology, 17, 237–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E. (1985). Developmental origins of motor coordination: leg movements in human infants. Developmental Psychology, 18, 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E. (1986). Treadmill-elicited stepping in seven-month-old infants. Child Development, 57, 1498–1506.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E., & Fisher, D. M. (1982). Newborn stepping: An explanation for a “disappearing reflex.” Developmental Psychology, 18, 760–775.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E., & Fisher, D. M. (1983a). From spontaneous to instrumental behavior: Kinematic analysis of movement changes during very early learning. Child Development, 54, 129–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E., & Fisher, D. M. (1983b). The organization of spontaneous leg movements in newborn infants. Journal of Motor Behavior, 15, 353–377.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E., Bradshaw, G., & Ward, J. A. (1981). Spontaneous kicking in month-old infants: manifestations of a human central locomotor program. Behavioral and Neural Biology, 32, 45–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E., Ridley-Johnson, R., & Fisher, D. M. (1983). Shifting patterns of bilateral coordination and lateral dominance in the leg movements of young infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 16, 29–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E., Fisher, D. M., & Ridley-Johnson, R. (1984). The relationship between physical growth and a newborn reflex. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 479–493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E., Kelso, J. A. S., and Fogel, A. (1986). Self-organizing systems and infant motor development. Developmental Review, 7, 39–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E., Skala, K., & Kelso, J. A. S. (1987). The dynamic nature of early coordination: Evidence from bilateral leg movements in young infants. Developmental Psychology, 23, 179–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1977). Descriptive analyses of infant communication behaviour. In H. R. Schaffer (Ed.), Studies in mother-infant interaction, (pp. 227–270 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in primary inter-subjectivity. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before speech, (pp. 321–348 ). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tuller, B., Turvey, M. T., & Fitch, H. (1982). The Bernstein perspective. II. The concept of muscle linkage or coordinative structure. In J. A. S. Kelso (Ed.), Human motor behavior: An Introduction, (pp. 252–270 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turvey, M. T. (1977). Preliminaries to a theory of action with a reference to vision. In R. Shaw & J. Bransford (Eds.), Perceiving, acting, and knowing: Toward an ecological psychology, (pp. 211–265 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waddington, C. H. (1966). Principles of development and differentiation. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wapner, S., & Kaplan, B. (1983). Toward a holistic developmental psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Webster, G., & Goodwin, B. (1981). History and structure in biology. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, 25, 39–62.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Werner, H. (1957). The concept of development from a comparative and organismic point of view. In D. B. Harris (Ed.). The concept of development, (pp. 125–148 ). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werner, J. (1977). Mathematical treatment of structure and function of human thermoregulatory system. Biology Cybernetics, 25, 93–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whiting, H. T. A. (1984). Human motor actions: Bernstein reassessed. Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winfree, A. T. (1980). The geometry of biological time. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolff, P. H. (1967). The role of biological rhythms in early psychological development. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 31, 197–218.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. R. (1983). The development of walking: New findings and old assumptions. Journal of Motor Behavior, 15, 99–137.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. R., Zelazo, N. A., & Kolb, S. (1972). “Walking” in the newborn. Science, 177, 1058–1059.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zeskind, P. S., & Lester, B. M. (1978). Acoustic features of auditory perceptions of the cries of newborns with prenatal and perinatal complications. Child Development, 49, 580–589.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1989 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Thelen, E., Fogel, A. (1989). Toward an Action-Based Theory of Infant Development. In: Lockman, J.J., Hazen, N.L. (eds) Action in Social Context. Perspectives in Developmental Psychology. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-9000-9_2

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-9000-9_2

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4757-9002-3

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4757-9000-9

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics