Advertisement

Behavior and Social Issues

, Volume 6, Issue 1, pp 35–54 | Cite as

What’s Wrong With Evolutionary Explanations of Human Behavior

  • Henry D. SchlingerJr.Email author
Article

Abstract

Recent books and articles have made strong claims for an evolutionary explanation of various human behaviors. These writings may be classified according to two positions on the evolution of human behavior. According to the first position, differences in behavior between groups of humans, (e.g., in intelligence) are said to be due to genetic differences. According to the second position, behavioral similarities among humans (e.g., in mating and aggression) are said to be due to genetic similarities. The evidence for these evolutionary explanations consists usually of a combination of evolutionary logic supported by general observations or correlations, behavioral analogies and comparisons with nonhuman species, and statistical analyses of data generated by non-experimental research methods. This paper presents a critique of this evidence and concludes that it is flawed scientifically and, therefore, it is insufficient to support an evolutionary explanation of human behavior. The paper concludes that environmental explanations represent an often more parsimonious alternative to evolutionary explanations, and, consequently, behavior analysts are in the best position to offer practical interventions for many ofthe behavioral problems that beset society.

Keywords

Human behavior evolutionary theories sociobiology evolutionary psychology behavior analysis 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Barash, D. (1977). Sociobiology and human behavior. New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  2. Buss, D. M. (1994). The evolution of desire: Strategies of human mating. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  3. Byne, W. (1994). The biological evidence challenged. Scientific American, 270, 50–55.  https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican0594-50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Chance, P. (1995). Learning and behavior (3rd. ed). Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks-Cole.Google Scholar
  5. Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1987). From evolution to behavior: Evolutionary psychology as the missing link. In J. Dupre (Ed.), The latest on the best essays on evolution and optimality. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  6. Daly, M, & Wilson, M. (1982). Whom are newborn babies said to resemble?. Ethology and Sociobiology, 3, 69–78.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0162-3095(82)90002-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Daly, M, & Wilson, M. (1988). Homicide. New York: Aldine De Gruyter.Google Scholar
  8. Davey, G. C. L., Forster, L., & Mayhew, G. (1993). Familial resemblances in disgust sensitivity and animal phobias. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 41–50.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(93)90041-RCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Futuyma, D. J. (1979). Evolutionary biology. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.Google Scholar
  10. Gould, S. J. (1981). The mismeasure of man. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
  11. Hart, B, & Risley, T. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: Paul Brookes.Google Scholar
  12. Herrnstein, R. J., & Murray, C. (1994). The bell curve: Intelligence and class structure in American life. New York: The Free Press.Google Scholar
  13. Horgan, J. Eugenics revisited. (1993). Scientific American, 268, 123–131.  https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican0493-123CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Hubbard, R., & Wald, E. (1993). Exploding the gene myth. Boston: Beacon Press.Google Scholar
  15. Itzkoff, S. W. (1994). The decline of intelligence in America: A strategy for national renewal. Westport, CT: Praeger.Google Scholar
  16. Jensen, A. R. (1969). How much can we boost IQ and scholastic achievement?. Harvard Educational Review, 39, 1–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Kamin, L. (1974). The science and politics of IQ. Potomac, MD: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  18. Kellert, S. R., & Wilson, E. O. (1993). The biophilia hypothesis. Washington, DC: Island Press.Google Scholar
  19. Kitcher, P. (1985). Vaulting ambition: Sociobiology and the quest for human nature. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  20. Levis, D. G. (1979). A reconsideration of Eysenck’s conditioning model of neurosis. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 2, 172–174.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00061732CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Levis, D. G., & Malloy, P. F. (1982). Research in infrahuman and human conditioning. In G. T. Wilson & C. M. Franks (Eds.), Contemporary behavior therapy: Conceptual and empirical foundations. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  22. Lewontin, R. C, Rose, S., & Kamin, L. J. (1984). Not in our genes. New York: Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
  23. Lubow, R. E., & Moore, A. U. (1959). Latent inhibition: The effect of non-reinforced preexposure to the conditioned stimulus. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 52, 415–419.  https://doi.org/10.1037/h0046700CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. McNally, R. J. (1987). Preparedness and phobias: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 283–303.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.101.2.283CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Mineka, S. (1985). The frightful complexity of the origins of fears. In F. R. Brush & J. B. Overmier (Eds.), Affect, conditioning, and cognition. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  26. Mineka, S., & Cook, M. (1986). Immunization against observational conditioning of snake fears in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 307–318.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.95.4.307CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Neale, J. M., & Liebert, R. M. (1973). Science and behavior: An introduction to methods of research. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  28. Ohman, A., & Soares, J. J. F. (1992). On the automatic nature of phobic fear: Conditioned electrodermal responses to masked fear-relevant stimuli. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,Google Scholar
  29. Rose, R. J., Miller, J. Z., Pogue-Geile, M. F., & Cardwell, G. F. (1981). Twin-family studies of common fears and phobias. In L. Gedda, L. Parisi, & W. E. Nance (Eds.), Progress in clinical and biological research (Vol. 69B). New York: Alan R. Liss.Google Scholar
  30. Rushton, J. P. (1995a). Race, evolution, and behavior: A life history perspective. New Brunswick: Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
  31. Rushton, J. P. (1995b). J. Philippe Rushton responds.. Skeptic, 3, 22–25.Google Scholar
  32. Scarr, S. (1968). Environmental bias in twin studies. In S. G. Vandenberg (Ed.) Progress in human behavior genetics. Baltimore: John Hopkins Press.Google Scholar
  33. Schlinger, H. D. (1992). Intelligence: Real or artificial?. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 10, 125–133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  35. Skinner, B. F. (1972). A case history in scientific method. In B. F. Skinner, Cumulative record: A selection of papers (3rd ed., pp. 101–124). New York:Google Scholar
  36. Soares, J. J. F., & Ohman, A. (1993). Preattentive processing, preparedness and phobias: Effects of instructions on conditioned electrodermal responses to masked and non-masked fear-relevant stimuli. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 87–95.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(93)90046-WCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Symons, D. (1992). On the use and misuse of Darwinism in the study of human behavior. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  38. Tavris, C. (1995). A place in the sun. Skeptic, 3, 62–63.Google Scholar
  39. Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1989). Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture, Part n. Ethology and Sociobiology, 10, 51–97.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0162-3095(89)90012-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  41. Torgersen, S. (1979). The nature and origin of common phobic fears. British Journal of Psychiatry, 134. 343–351.  https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.134.4.343CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Wilson, E. O. (1993). Biophilia and the conservation ethic. In S.R. Kellert & E.O. Wilson (Eds.), The biophilia hypothesis. Washington, D.C.: Island Press.Google Scholar
  43. Wilson, E. O. (1984). Biophilia: The human bond with other species. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  44. Wright, R. (1994). The moral animal: The new science of evolutionary psychology. New York: Pantheon.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Association for Behavior Analysis International 1996

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyWestern New England CollegeSpringfieldUSA

Personalised recommendations