Advertisement

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review

, Volume 26, Issue 5, pp 1472–1502 | Cite as

Emotional oddball: A review on memory effects

  • Helge SchlüterEmail author
  • Ryan P. Hackländer
  • Christina Bermeitinger
Theoretical Review
  • 242 Downloads

Abstract

How do emotional stimuli influence perception, attention, and ultimately memory? This debate at the cross-section of emotion and cognition research has a long tradition. The emotional oddball paradigm (EOP) has frequently been applied to investigate the detection and processing of (emotional) change detection (Schlüter & Bermeitinger, 2017). However, the EOP has also been used to reveal the effects of emotional deviants on memory for serially presented stimuli. In this integrative article, we review the results of 29 experiments published between the years 2000 and 2017. Based on these data, we provide an overview of how the EOP is applied in the context of memory research. We also review and integrate the empirical evidence for memory effects in the EOP (with a special focus on retrograde and anterograde emotion-induced effects) and present theories of emotional memory as well as their fit with the results obtained by the EOP. Directions for future research are presented that would help to address important issues of the current debate around emotion-induced memory effects.

Keywords

Emotional oddball Emotional stimuli Memory Deviation Expectation 

Notes

References

  1. Adelman, J. S., & Estes, Z. (2013). Emotion and memory: A recognition advantage for positive and negative words independent of arousal. Cognition, 129(3), 530-535. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.08.014 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Anderson, A. K., Wais, P. E., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2006). Emotion enhances remembrance of neutral events past. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(5), 1599-1604. doi: https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0506308103 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bayer, M., Sommer, W., & Schacht, A. (2012). P1 and beyond: Functional separation of multiple emotion effects in word recognition. Psychophysiology, 49(7), 959-969. doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01381.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Beck, D. M., & Kastner, S. (2009). Top-down and bottom-up mechanisms in biasing competition in the human brain. Vision research, 49(10), 1154-1165. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2008.07.012 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1999). Affective norms for English words (ANEW): Instruction manual and affective ratings (Vol. 30, No. 1, pp. 25-36). Technical report C-1, the center for research in psychophysiology, University of Florida.Google Scholar
  6. Bradley, M. M., Greenwald, M. K., Petry, M. C., & Lang, P. J. (1992). Remembering pictures: pleasure and arousal in memory. Journal of experimental psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(2), 379. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.18.2.379 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Briggs, K. E. & Martin, F. H. (2009). Affective picture processing and motivational relevance: arousal and valence effects on ERPs in an oddball task. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 72(3), 299-306. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.01.009 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Cahill, L., Prins, B., Weber, M., & McGaugh, J. L. (1994). β-Adrenergic activation and memory for emotional events. Nature, 371(6499), 702-704. doi:10.1038/371702a0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. *Clewett, D., Sakaki, M., Nielsen, S., Petzinger, G., & Mather, M. (2017). Noradrenergic mechanisms of arousal’s bidirectional effects on episodic memory. Neurobiology of learning and memory, 137, 1-14. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2016.10.017 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Detterman, D. K. (1976). The retrieval hypothesis as an explanation of induced retrograde amnesia. The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology, 28(4), 623-632. doi:10.1080/14640747608400588CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Detterman, D. K. & Ellis, N. R. (1972). Determinants of induced amnesia in short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 95(2), 308-316. doi:10.1037/h0033629CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Dolcos, F., LaBar, K. S., & Cabeza, R. (2004). Dissociable effects of arousal and valence on prefrontal activity indexing emotional evaluation and subsequent memory: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage, 23(1), 64-74. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.05.015 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Ellis, N. R., Detterman, D. K., Runcie, D., McCarver, R. B., & Craig, E. M. (1971). Amnesic effects in short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 89(2), 357-361. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/h0031192 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. *Froeliger, B., Gilbert, D. G., & McClernon, F. J. (2009). Effects of nicotine on novelty detection and memory recognition performance: Double-blind, placebo-controlled studies of smokers and nonsmokers. Psychopharmacology, 205, 625-633. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-009-1571-y CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Gable, P. A. & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). The effect of low versus high approach-motivated positive affect on memory for peripherally versus centrally presented information. Emotion, 10(4), 599-603. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018426 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Hamann, S. (2001). Cognitive and neural mechanisms of emotional memory. Trends in cognitive sciences, 5(9), 394-400. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01707-1 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Harmon-Jones, E., Harmon-Jones, C., & Price, T. F. (2013). What is approach motivation? Emotion Review, 5(3), 291-295. doi: https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073913477509 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hinojosa, J. A., Carretié, L., Valcárcel, M. A., Méndez-Bértolo, C., & Pozo, M. A. (2009). Electrophysiological differences in the processing of affective information in words and pictures. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 9(2), 173-189. doi: https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.9.2.173 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hurlemann, R. (2008). Noradrenergic–glucocorticoid mechanisms in emotion-induced amnesia: from adaptation to disease. Psychopharmacology, 197(1), 13-23. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-007-1002-x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. *Hurlemann, R., Hawellek, B., Maier, W., & Dolan, R. J. (2007a). Enhanced emotion-induced amnesia in borderline personality disorder. Psychological Medicine, 37, 971-981. doi: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291706009792 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. *Hurlemann, R., Hawellek, B., Matusch, A., Kolsch, H., Wollersen, H., Madea, B., ... & Dolan, R. J. (2005). Noradrenergic modulation of emotion-induced forgetting and remembering. The Journal of Neuroscience, 25(27), 6343-6349. doi: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0228-05.2005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. *Hurlemann, R., Wagner, M., Hawellek, B., Reich, H., Pieperhoff, P., Amunts, K., … & Dolan, R. J. (2007b). Amygdala control of emotion-induced forgetting and remembering: Evidence from Urbach-Wiethe disease. Neuropsychologia, 45, 877-884. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.08.027 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Ioannidis, J. P. (2008). Why most discovered true associations are inflated. Epidemiology, 19(5), 640-648. doi: https://doi.org/10.1097/EDE.0b013e31818131e7 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. *Kamp, S. M., Potts, G. F., & Donchin, E. (2015). On the roles of distinctiveness and semantic expectancies in episodic encoding of emotional words. Psychophysiology, 52(12), 1599-1609. doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12537 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Kensinger, E. A. (2009). Remembering the details: Effects of emotion. Emotion review, 1(2), 99-113. doi: https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073908100432 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  26. Kensinger, E. A., & Corkin, S. (2004). Two routes to emotional memory: Distinct neural processes for valence and arousal. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101(9), 3310-3315. doi: https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0306408101 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Kensinger, E. A., Garoff-Eaton, R. J., & Schacter, D. L. (2006). Memory for specific visual details can be enhanced by negative arousing content. Journal of Memory and Language, 54(1), 99-112. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2005.05.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Kensinger, E. A. & Schacter, D. L. (2006). When the Red Sox shocked the Yankees: Comparing negative and positive memories. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(5), 757-763. doi: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193993 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. *Knight, M. & Mather, M. (2009). Reconciling findings of emotion-induced memory enhancement and impairment of preceding items. Emotion, 9(6), 763-781. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017281 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. *Kroes, M. C., Strange, B. A., & Dolan, R. J. (2010). β-adrenergic blockade during memory retrieval in humans evokes a sustained reduction of declarative emotional memory enhancement. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(11), 3959-3963. doi: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5469-09.2010 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1999). International affective picture system (IAPS): Instruction manual and affective ratings. Gainesville, FL: The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.Google Scholar
  32. Lundqvist, D., Flykt, A., & Öhman, A. (1998). The Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces – KDEF, CD ROM from Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Psychology section, Karolinska Institutet, ISBN 91-630-7164-9.Google Scholar
  33. Madigan, S. (1983). Picture memory. In J. Yuille (ed.), Imagery, memory, and cognition: Essays in honor of Allan Paivio (pp.65-89). New York (NY): Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  34. Markovic, J., Anderson, A. K., & Todd, R. M. (2014). Tuning to the significant: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of visual perception and memory. Behavioural brain research, 259, 229-241. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2013.11.018 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Mather, M., & Nesmith, K. (2008). Arousal-enhanced location memory for pictures. Journal of memory and language, 58(2), 449-464. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.01.004 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  36. Mather, M. & Sutherland, M. R. (2011). Arousal-biased competition in perception and memory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(2), 114–133. doi: https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691611400234 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  37. Mather, M., Clewett, D., Sakaki, M., & Harley, C. W. (2016). Norepinephrine ignites local hotspots of neuronal excitation: How arousal amplifies selectivity in perception and memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39. doi: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X15000667
  38. *Miu, A. C., Heilman, R. M., Opre, A., & Miclea, M. (2005). Emotion-induced retrograde amnesia and trait anxiety. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 1250-1257. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.31.6.1250 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. *Müller, N. G., Wohlrath, B., Kopp, U. A., & Lengler, U. (2009). Emotional content does not interfere with verbal memory in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy & Behavior, 15(3), 367-371. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2009.05.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Ogden, C.K. (1932). Basic English: A general introduction with rules and grammar (3rd ed.). London: K. Paul, Trench, Trubner & Co., Ltd.Google Scholar
  41. Paivio, A. (1969). Mental imagery in associative learning and memory. Psychological review, 76(3), 241-263. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/h0027272 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Richardson, M. P., Strange, B. A., & Dolan, R. J. (2004a). Encoding of emotional memories depends on amygdala and hippocampus and their interactions. Nature neuroscience, 7(3), 278-285. doi: https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1190 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Richardson, M. P., Strange, B. A., Duncan, J. S., & Dolan, R. J. (2003). Preserved verbal memory function in left medial temporal pathology involves reorganisation of function to right medial temporal lobe. Neuroimage, 20, S112-S119. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.09.008 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Richardson, M. P., Strange, B. A., Duncan, J. S., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Memory fMRI in left hippocampal sclerosis optimizing the approach to predicting postsurgical memory. Neurology, 66(5), 699-705. doi: https://doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000201186.07716.98 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  45. Richardson, M. P., Strange, B. A., Thompson, P. J., Baxendale, S. A., 1933 Duncan, J. S., & Dolan, R. J. (2004b). Pre-operative verbal memory 1934 fMRI predicts post-operative memory decline after left temporal 1935 lobe resection. Brain, 127(11), 2419-2426. doi: https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awh293 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Roediger III, H. L. (2008). Relativity of remembering: Why the laws of memory vanished. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 225-254. doi: https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190139 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Runcie, D., & O'Bannon, R. M. (1977). An independence of induced amnesia and emotional response. The American journal of psychology, 55-61. http://www.jstor.org/stable/1421640
  48. *Sakaki, M., Fryer, K., & Mather, M. (2014). Emotion strengthens high-priority memory traces but weakens low-priority memory traces. Psychological Science, 25(2), 387-395. doi: https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613504784 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Saufley, W. H., Jr. & Winograd, E. (1970). Retrograde amnesia and priority instructions in free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 85(1), 150-152. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/h0029538 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Schlüter, H. & Bermeitinger, C. (2017). Emotional oddball: A review on variants, results, and mechanisms. Review of General Psychology, 21(3), 179-222. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/gpr0000120 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Schmidt, S. R. (2002). Outstanding memories: The positive and negative effects of nudes on memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(2), 353-361. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.28.2.353 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Schmidt, S. R. & Schmidt, C. R. (2016). The emotional carryover effect in memory for words. Memory, 24(7), 916-938. doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2015.1059859 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Schultz, L. S. (1971). Effects of high priority events on recall and recognition of other items. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10, 322–330. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(71)80062-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. *Smith, R. M. & Beversdorf, D. Q. (2008). Effects of semantic relatedness on recall of stimuli preceding emotional oddballs. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14, 620-628. doi: https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617708080843 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Snodgrass, J. G. & Vanderwart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of experimental psychology: Human learning and memory, 6(2), 174-215.Google Scholar
  56. Strange, B. A. & Dolan, R. J. (2001). Adaptive anterior hippocampal responses to oddball stimuli. Hippocampus, 11, 690-698. doi: https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.1084 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. *Strange, B. A. & Dolan, R. J. (2004). β-Adrenergic modulation of emotional memory-evoked human amygdala and hippocampal responses. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101, 11454-11458. doi: https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0404282101 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Strange, B. A., & Dolan, R. J. (2007). β-adrenergic modulation of oddball responses in humans. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 3-29. doi: https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-3-29 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Strange, B., & Galarza-Vallejo, A. (2016). Bidirectional synaptic plasticity can explain bidirectional retrograde effects of emotion on memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, E224. doi: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X15001958 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. *Strange, B. A., Gartmann, N., Brenninkmeyer, J., Haaker, J., Reif, A., Kalisch, R., & Büchel, C. (2014). Dopamine receptor 4 promoter polymorphism modulates memory and neuronal responses to salience. Neuroimage, 84, 922-931. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.09.065 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. *Strange, B. A., Henson, R. N. A., Friston, K. J., & Dolan, R. J. (2000). Brain mechanisms for detecting perceptual, semantic, and emotional deviance. Neuroimage, 12(4), 425-433. doi: https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2000.0637 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. *Strange, B. A., Hurlemann, R., & Dolan, R. J. (2003). An emotion-induced retrograde amnesia in humans is amygdala- and β-adrenergic dependent. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100, 13626-13631. doi: https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1635116100 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. *Strange, B. A., Kroes, M. C., Fan, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2010). Emotion causes targeted forgetting of established memories. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 4, 175. doi: https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00175
  64. *Strange, B. A., Kroes, M. C., Roiser, J. P., Tan, G. C., & Dolan, R. J. (2008). Emotion-induced retrograde amnesia is determined by a 5-HTT genetic polymorphism. Journal of Neuroscience, 28(28), 7036-7039. doi: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0834-08.2008 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Tarr, M. J. The object databank. 2005. http://www.tarrlab.org/
  66. Tulving E. (1969). Retrograde amnesia in free recall. Science, 164, 88–90. doi: https://doi.org/10.1126/science.164.3875.88 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Von Restorff, H. (1933). Über die wirkung von bereichsbildungen im spurenfeld. Psychologische Forschung, 18(1), 299-342. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02409636 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Wallace, W. P. (1965). Review of the historical, empirical, and theoretical status of the Von Restorff phenomenon. Psychological bulletin, 63(6), 410-424. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/h0022001 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  69. *West Saxvig, I., Johansen Lundervold, A., Grønli, J., Ursin, R., Bjorvatin, B., & Portas, C. M. (2008). The effect of a REM sleep deprivation procedure on different aspects of memory function in humans. Psychophysiology, 45, 309-317. doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00623.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Wolfe, J. M. (2003) Moving towards solutions to some enduring controversies in visual search. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 70-76. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00024-4 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© The Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  • Helge Schlüter
    • 1
    Email author
  • Ryan P. Hackländer
    • 1
  • Christina Bermeitinger
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Psychology, Experimental PsychologyUniversity of HildesheimHildesheimGermany

Personalised recommendations