Age-related differences in recall and recognition: a meta-analysis
- 322 Downloads
Abstract
Relative to younger adults, older adults tend to perform more poorly on tests of both free recall and item recognition memory. The age difference in performance is typically larger for recall tasks relative to those involving recognition. However, there have been reports of comparable age-related differences in free recall and item recognition performance. Further, a differential performance cost does not necessarily mean that processes involved in recall are specifically affected by age. Here we present a meta-analysis of 36 articles reporting 89 direct comparisons of free recall and item recognition in younger and older groups of participants. Standardized effect sizes reveal that age differences are larger for recall tasks (Hedges’ g = 0.89, 95% confidence intervals [0.75, 1.03]) than for recognition tasks (0.54, [0.37, 0.72]). Further, Brinley analyses of the data suggest that distinct functions are needed to relate younger and older performance for the two tasks. These functions differ in intercept pointing to a disproportionate age difference in recall relative to recognition. This is in line with theories of memory and aging which posit specific deficits in processes related to search and retrieval from memory.
Keywords
Recall Recognition Episodic memory Cognitive agingNotes
References
- Adamowicz, J., & Hudson, B. (1978). Visual short-term memory, response delay, and age. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 46(1), 267–270.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Ahmad, F. N., Fernandes, M., & Hockley, W. E. (2015). Improving associative memory in older adults with unitization. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 22(4), 452–472.Google Scholar
- Atkinson, R. C., & Juola, J. F. (1974). Search and decision processes in recognition memory. In D. H. Krantz, R. C. Atkinson, R. D. Luce, & P. Suppes (Eds.) Contemporary developments in mathematical psychology (vol. 1): Learning, memory, and thinking (pp. 239–289). San Francisco: Freeman.Google Scholar
- Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390–412.Google Scholar
- Bastin, C., Diana, R. A., Simon, J., Collette, F., Yonelinas, A. P., & Salmon, E. (2013). Associative memory in aging: The effect of unitization on source memory. Psychology and Aging, 28(1), 275.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Benjamin, A. S. (2010). Representational explanations of “process” dissociations in recognition: The DRYAD theory of aging and memory judgments. Psychological Review, 117(4), 1055–1079.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Bohn, L., See, S. T. K., & Fung, H. H. (2016). Time perspective and positivity effects in Alzheimer’s disease. Psychology and Aging, 31(6), 574–582.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Bopp, K. L., & Verhaeghen, P. (2005). Aging and verbal memory span: A meta-analysis. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 60(5), P223–P233.Google Scholar
- Botwinick, J., & Storandt, M. (1980). Recall and recognition of old information in relation to age and sex. Journal of Gerontology, 35(1), 70–76.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Brainerd, C., & Reyna, V. (2015). Fuzzy-trace theory and lifespan cognitive development. Developmental Review, 38, 89–121.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Brainerd, C., Reyna, V., & Howe, M. L. (2009). Trichotomous processes in early memory development, aging, and neurocognitive impairment: A unified theory. Psychological Review, 116(4), 783.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Brinley, J. F. (1965). Cognitive sets, speed and accuracy of performance in the elderly. Behavior, Aging and the Nervous System, 114–149.Google Scholar
- Burack, O. R., & Lachman, M. E. (1996). The effects of list-making on recall in young and elderly adults. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 51(4), P226–P233.Google Scholar
- Bürkner, P.-C. (2017). Brms: An R package for Bayesian multilevel models using Stan. Journal of Statistical Software, 80(1), 1–28.Google Scholar
- Bürkner, P. -C. (2018). Advanced Bayesian multilevel modeling with the R package brms. The R Journal, 10 (1), 395–411.Google Scholar
- Carpenter, B., Gelman, A., Hoffman, M., Lee, D., Goodrich, B., Betancourt, M. …, & et al. (2016). Stan: A probabilistic programming language. Journal of Statistical Software, 20(2), 1–37.Google Scholar
- Carrier, M., & Pashler, H. (1992). The influence of retrieval on retention. Memory & Cognition, 20(6), 633–642.Google Scholar
- Cerella, J. (1985). Information processing rates in the elderly. Psychological Bulletin, 98(1), 67.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Charles, S. T., Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2003). Aging and emotional memory: The forgettable nature of negative images for older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132(2), 310.Google Scholar
- Clark, H. H. (1973). The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: a critique of language statistics in psychological research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12(4), 335–359.Google Scholar
- Coane, J. H. (2013). Retrieval practice and elaborative encoding benefit memory in younger and older adults. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 2(2), 95–100.Google Scholar
- Cox, G. E., Hemmer, P., Aue, W. R., & Criss, A. H. (2018). Information and processes underlying semantic and episodic memory across tasks, items, and individuals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(4), 545.Google Scholar
- Craik, F. I. M. (1983). On the transfer of information from temporary to permanent memory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 302(1110), 341–359.Google Scholar
- Craik, F. I. M., & McDowd, J. M. (1987). Age differences in recall and recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13(3), 474.Google Scholar
- Danckert, S. L., & Craik, F. I. M. (2013). Does aging affect recall more than recognition memory? Psychology and Aging, 28(4), 902–909.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Davis, H. P., Cohen, A., Gandy, M., Colombo, P., VanDusseldorp, G., Simolke, N., & Romano, J. (1990). Lexical priming deficits as a function of age. Behavioral Neuroscience, 104(2), 288.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Delhaye, E., Folville, A., & Bastin, C. (2019). How to induce an age-related benefit of semantic relatedness in associative memory: It’s all in the design. Psychology and Aging, 34(4), 572.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Dunn, J. C. (2004). Remember-know: a matter of confidence. Psychological Review, 111(2), 524–542.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Egger, M., Smith, G. D., Schneider, M., & Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ, 315(7109), 629–634.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Erber, J. (1974). Age differences in recognition memory. Journal of Gerontology, 29(2), 177–181.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Erber, J., Herman, T. G., & Botwinick, J. (1980). Age differences in memory as a function of depth of processing. Experimental Aging Research, 6(4), 341–348.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Fraundorf, S. H., Hourihan, K. L., Peters, R. A., & Benjamin, A. S. (2019). Aging and recognition memory: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, online ahead of print.Google Scholar
- Freeman, E., Heathcote, A., Chalmers, K., & Hockley, W. (2010). Item effects in recognition memory for words. Journal of Memory and Language, 62(1), 1–18.Google Scholar
- Gelman, A., Carlin, J. B., Stern, H. S., Dunson, D. B., Vehtari, A., & Rubin, D. B. (2014) Bayesian data analysis Vol. 3. Boca Raton: Chapman & Hall/CRC.Google Scholar
- Gillard, J. (2010). An overview of linear structural models in errors in variables regression. REVSTAT–Statistical Journal, 8(1), 57–80.Google Scholar
- Gordon, S. K., & Clark, W. C. (1974). Application of signal detection theory to prose recall and recognition in elderly and young adults. Journal of Gerontology, 29(1), 64–72.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Gronau, Q. F., & Singmann, H. (2017). Bridgesampling: Bridge sampling for marginal likelihoods and Bayes factors. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=bridgesampling
- Gronau, Q. F., Sarafoglou, A., Matzke, D., Ly, A., Boehm, U., Marsman, M., & Steingroever, H. … (2017). A tutorial on bridge sampling. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 81, 80–97.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Haaf, J. M., Rhodes, S., Sun, T. K., Snyder, H. K., Naveh-Benjamin, M., & Rouder, J. (2018). Revisiting the remember-know task: Replications of Gardiner and Java (1990). Retrieved from https://psyarxiv.com/2dyn7/
- Haaland, K. Y., Price, L., & Larue, A. (2003). What does the WMS–III tell us about memory changes with normal aging? Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 9(1), 89–96.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Harwood, E., & Naylor, G. (1969). Recall and recognition in elderly and young subjects. Australian Journal of Psychology, 21(3), 251–257.Google Scholar
- Healey, M. K., & Kahana, M. J. (2016). A four-component model of age-related memory change. Psychological Review, 123(1), 23–69.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Hedges, L. V. (1981). Distribution theory for glass’s estimator of effect size and related estimators. Journal of Educational Statistics, 6(2), 107–128.Google Scholar
- Howard, D. V. (1983). The effects of aging and degree of association on the semantic priming of lexical decisions. Experimental Aging Research, 9(3), 145–151.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Howard, M. W., Bessette-Symons, B., Zhang, Y., & Hoyer, W. J. (2006). Aging selectively impairs recollection in recognition memory for pictures: Evidence from modeling and receiver operating characteristic curves. Psychology and Aging, 21(1), 96.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Ioannidis, J. P., Munafo, M. R., Fusar-Poli, P., Nosek, B. A., & David, S. P. (2014). Publication and other reporting biases in cognitive sciences: Detection, prevalence, and prevention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18 (5), 235–241.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Jaroslawska, A. J., & Rhodes, S. (2019). Adult age differences in the effects of processing on storage in working memory: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 34(4), 512.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Jennings, J. M., & Jacoby, L. L. (1993). Automatic versus intentional uses of memory: Aging, attention, and control. Psychology and Aging, 8(2), 283–293.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Jin, Z.-C., Zhou, X.-H., & He, J. (2015). Statistical methods for dealing with publication bias in meta-analysis. Statistics in Medicine, 34(2), 343–360.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Kalenzaga, S., Lamidey, V., Ergis, A.-M., Clarys, D., & Piolino, P. (2016). The positivity bias in aging: Motivation or degradation? Emotion, 16(5), 602–610.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Karrasch, M., Myllyniemi, A., Latvasalo, L., Söderholm, C., Ellfolk, U., & Laine, M. (2010). The diagnostic accuracy of an incidental memory modification of the Boston Naming Test (memo-BNT) in differentiating between normal aging and mild Alzheimer’s disease. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 24(8), 1355–1364.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Knopf, M., & Neidhardt, E. (1989). Aging and memory for action events: The role of familiarity. Developmental Psychology, 25(5), 780.Google Scholar
- Koutstaal, W., & Schacter, D. L. (1997). Gist-based false recognition of pictures in older and younger adults. Journal of Memory and Language, 37(4), 555–583.Google Scholar
- Koutstaal, W., Schacter, D. L., Galluccio, L., & Stofer, K. A. (1999). Reducing gist-based false recognition in older adults: Encoding and retrieval manipulations. Psychology and Aging, 14(2), 220.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Kramer, J. H., Blusewicz, M. J., & Preston, K. A. (1989). The premature aging hypothesis: Old before its time? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57(2), 257.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- La Voie, D., & Light, L. L. (1994). Adult age differences in repetition priming: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 9(4), 539–553.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Lalanne, J., Rozenberg, J., Grolleau, P., & Piolino, P. (2013). The self-reference effect on episodic memory recollection in young and older adults and Alzheimer’s disease. Current Alzheimer Research, 10(10), 1107–1117.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Leigland, L. A., Schulz, L. E., & Janowsky, J. S. (2004). Age-related changes in emotional memory. Neurobiology of Aging, 25(8), 1117–1124.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Lewandowski, D., Kurowicka, D., & Joe, H. (2009). Generating random correlation matrices based on vines and extended onion method. Journal of Multivariate Analysis, 100(9), 1989–2001.Google Scholar
- Li, S. C., Naveh-Benjamin, M., & Lindenberger, U. (2005). Aging neuromodulation impairs associative binding: A neurocomputational account. Psychological Science, 16(6), 445–450.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Light, L. L., & Anderson, P. A. (1983). Memory for scripts in young and older adults. Memory & Cognition, 11(5), 435–444.Google Scholar
- Light, L. L., & Singh, A. (1987). Implicit and explicit memory in young and older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13(4), 531.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Loftus, G. R. (1978). On interpretation of interactions. Memory & Cognition, 6(3), 312–319.Google Scholar
- Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (2005) Detection theory: A user’s guide, (2nd edn.) Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
- Madden, D. J. (1986). Adult age differences in visual word recognition: Semantic encoding and episodic retention. Experimental Aging Research, 12(2), 71–78.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Mandler, G. (1980). Recognizing: The judgment of previous occurrence. Psychological Review, 87(3), 252–271.Google Scholar
- May, C. P., Hasher, L., & Stoltzfus, E. R. (1993). Optimal time of day and the magnitude of age differences in memory. Psychological Science, 4(5), 326–330.Google Scholar
- McNicol, D. (1972) A primer of signal detection theory. London: George Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar
- Meyer, A. N., & Logan, J. M. (2013). Taking the testing effect beyond the college freshman: Benefits for lifelong learning. Psychology and Aging, 28(1), 142.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Murphy, C., Nordin, S., & Acosta, L. (1997). Odor learning, recall, and recognition memory in young and elderly adults. Neuropsychology, 11(1), 126.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Myerson, J., Hale, S., Wagstaff, D., Poon, L. W., & Smith, G. A. (1990). The information-loss model: A mathematical theory of age-related cognitive slowing. Psychological Review, 97(4), 475.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Naveh-Benjamin, M. (2000). Adult age differences in memory performance: Tests of an associative deficit hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(5), 1170.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Naveh-Benjamin, M., Brav, T. K., & Levy, O. (2007). The associative memory deficit of older adults: The role of strategy utilization. Psychology and Aging, 22(1), 202–208.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Naveh-Benjamin, M., Craik, F. I. M., Guez, J., & Kreuger, S. (2005). Divided attention in younger and older adults: Effects of strategy and relatedness on memory performance and secondary task costs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(3), 520.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Naveh-Benjamin, M., Shing, Y. L., Kilb, A., Werkle-Bergner, M., Lindenberger, U., & Li, S.-C. (2009). Adult age differences in memory for name–face associations: The effects of intentional and incidental learning. Memory, 17(2), 220–232.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Neely, K. A., Samimy, S., Blouch, S. L., Wang, P., Chennavasin, A., Diaz, M. T., & Dennis, N. A. (2017). Memory-guided force control in healthy younger and older adults. Experimental Brain Research, 235(8), 2473–2482.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Nyberg, L., Maitland, S. B., Rönnlund, M., Bäckman, L., Dixon, R. A., Wahlin, Å, & Nilsson, L.-G. (2003). Selective adult age differences in an age-invariant multifactor model of declarative memory. Psychology and Aging, 18(1), 149–160.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Old, S. R., & Naveh-Benjamin, M. (2008). Differential effects of age on item and associative measures of memory: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 23(1), 104–118.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Pastötter, B., & Bäuml, K.-H.T. (2019). Testing enhances subsequent learning in older adults. Psychology and Aging, 34(2), 242.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Perlmutter, M. (1978). What is memory aging the aging of? Developmental Psychology, 14(4), 330.Google Scholar
- Perlmutter, M. (1979). Age differences in adults’ free recall, cued recall, and recognition. Journal of Gerontology, 34(4), 533–539.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Pratte, M. S., & Rouder, J. N. (2012). Assessing the dissociability of recollection and familiarity in recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology:, Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(6), 1591–1607.Google Scholar
- R Core Team. (2018) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from https://www.R-project.org/ Google Scholar
- Raaijmakers, J. G., & Shiffrin, R. M. (2002). Models of memory. In H. Pashler, & D. Medin (Eds.) Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology: Memory and cognitive processes. (3rd edn.) (pp. 43–76). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
- Rhodes, S., Cowan, N., Parra, M. A., & Logie, R. H. (2018). Interaction effects on common measures of sensitivity: Choice of measure, type i error, and power. Behavior Research Methods, online ahead of print.Google Scholar
- Riggs, D. S., Guarnieri, J. A., & Addelman, S. (1978). Fitting straight lines when both variables are subject to error. Life Sciences, 22(13-15), 1305–1360.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Roediger, H. L. III, & Karpicke, J. D. (2006). Test-enhanced learning: Taking memory tests improves long-term retention. Psychological Science, 17(3), 249–255.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Rohling, M. L., Ellis, N. R., & Scogin, F. (1991). Automatic and effortful memory processes in elderly persons with organic brain pathology. Journal of Gerontology, 46(4), P137–P143.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Rotello, C. M., & Heit, E. (2000). Associative recognition: A case of recall-to-reject processing. Memory & Cognition, 28(6), 907–922.Google Scholar
- Rouder, J. N., & Lu, J. (2005). An introduction to Bayesian hierarchical models with an application in the theory of signal detection. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(4), 573–604.Google Scholar
- Ruiz-Gallego-Largo, T., Simón, T., & Suengas, A. G. (2012). Age differences in source monitoring and referent discrimination. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 15(1), 20–28.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Russo, R., & Parkin, A. J. (1993). Age differences in implicit memory: More apparent than real. Memory & Cognition, 21(1), 73–80.Google Scholar
- Salthouse, T. A. (2000). Methodological assumptions in cognitive aging research. In F. I. M. Craik, & T. A. Salthouse (Eds.) The handbook of cognitive aging. (2nd edn.) (pp. 467–498). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
- Schmitter-Edgecombe, M., & Simpson, A. L. (2001). Effects of age and intentionality on content memory and temporal memory for performed activities. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 8(2), 81–97.Google Scholar
- Schonfield, D., & Robertson, B. A. (1966). Memory storage and aging. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 20 (2), 228–236.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Schonfield, D., Davidson, H., & Jones, H. (1983). An example of age-associated interference in memorizing. Journal of Gerontology, 38(2), 204–210.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Schramke, C. J., & Bauer, R. M. (1997). State-dependent learning in older and younger adults. Psychology and Aging, 12(2), 255.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Sliwinski, M. J., & Hall, C. B. (1998). Constraints on general slowing: A meta-analysis using hierarchical linear models with random coefficients. Psychology and Aging, 13(1), 164.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Sommers, M. S., & Huff, L. M. (2003). The effects of age and dementia of the Alzheimer’s type on phonological false memories. Psychology and Aging, 18(4), 791.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Spencer, W. D., & Raz, N. (1995). Differential effects of aging on memory for content and context: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 10(4), 527.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Spilich, G. J., & Voss, J. F. (1983). Contextual effects upon text memory for young, aged-normal, and aged memory-impaired individuals. Experimental Aging Research, 9(1), 45–49.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Surprenant, A. M., & Neath, I. (2009). Principles of memory. Psychology Press, Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
- Trelle, A. N., Henson, R. N., & Simons, J. S. (2015). Identifying age-invariant and age-limited mechanisms for enhanced memory performance: Insights from self-referential processing in younger and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 30(2), 324.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Uttl, B., Henry, M., & Baltimore, K. (2007). Are smaller age differences on old/new recognition versus free recall tests artifacts of easy memory tests? Canadian Journal of Psychology, 61, 374.Google Scholar
- Vakil, E., Melamed, M.-D., & Even, N. (1996). Direct and indirect measures of contextual information: Older versus young adult subjects. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 3(1), 30–36.Google Scholar
- Verhaeghen, P. (2013) The elements of cognitive aging: Meta-analyses of age-related differences in processing speed and their consequences. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
- Verhaeghen, P., & Marcoen, A. (1993). Memory aging as a general phenomenon: Episodic recall of older adults is a function of episodic recall of young adults. Psychology and Aging, 8(3), 380–388.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Verhaeghen, P., Steitz, D. W., Sliwinski, M. J., & Cerella, J. (2003). Aging and dual-task performance: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 18(3), 443–460.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Verhaeghen, P., Vandenbroucke, A., & Dierckx, V. (1998). Growing slower and less accurate: Adult age differences in time-accuracy functions for recall and recognition from episodic memory. Experimental Aging Research, 24, 3–19.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. Journal of Statistical Software, 36(3), 1–48. Retrieved from http://www.jstatsoft.org/v36/i03/ Google Scholar
- White, N., & Cunningham, W. R. (1982). What is the evidence for retrieval problems in the elderly? Experimental Aging Research, 8(3), 169–171.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Whiting, I., Wythe, L., & Smith, A. D. (1997). Differential age-related processing limitations in recall and recognition tasks. Psychology and Aging, 12(2), 216–224.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Yonelinas, A. P. (2002). The nature of recollection and familiarity: A review of 30 years of research. Journal of Memory and Language, 46(3), 441–517.Google Scholar
- Zelinski, E. M., & Miura, S. A. (1988). Effects of thematic information on script memory in young and old adults. Psychology and Aging, 3(3), 292.PubMedGoogle Scholar