Advertisement

Perspective taking in judgment of relative direction tasks

  • Danit GevaEmail author
  • Avishai Henik
Article
  • 23 Downloads

Abstract

Mental rotation and visual-spatial perspective taking (VSPT) are two visual-spatial abilities that are highly correlated. Judgment of relative direction (JRD) tasks are commonly used to examine VSPT in conjunction with mental rotation. Moreover, in many studies the task involves switching from an imagined perspective to a sensorimotor perspective at the response phase. The current study employed a JRD task that enabled examination of mental rotation, perspective taking, and perspective switching, separately. In two experiments we found that both perspective taking and perspective switching affected accuracy and response time (RT). Namely, trials with a perspective change larger than 90° (that required perspective taking) were harder than trials with a change smaller than 90° (that required mental rotation), and so were trials that required switching from an imagined perspective to a sensorimotor perspective. Importantly, we suggest that VSPT and mental rotation are dissociable.

Keywords

Mental transformation Perspective switching Visual-spatial perspective taking Mental rotation 

Notes

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by funding from the European Research Council under the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007–2013)/ERC Grant Agreement No. 295644. We would like to thank Ronen Hershman, Naama Katzin, and Desiree Meloul for their professional and generous help. Finally, we thank three research assistants – Tal Feldman, Matanel Weissmann, and Ram Orr – for their help.

References

  1. Avraamides, M. N., & Ioannidou, L. M. (2005). Locating targets from imagined perspectives: Labeling vs. pointing. In B. G. Bara, L. Barsalou, & M. Bucciarelli (Eds.), Proceedings of the 27 th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 175-180.Google Scholar
  2. Avraamides, M. N., & Kelly, J. W. (2008). Multiple systems of spatial memory and action. Cognitive Processing, 9(2), 93-106.Google Scholar
  3. Barratt, E. S. (1953). An analysis of verbal reports of solving spatial problems as an aid in defining spatial factors. The Journal of Psychology, 36(1), 17-25.Google Scholar
  4. Borich, G. D., & Bauman, P. M. (1972). Convergent and discriminant validation of the French and Guilford-Zimmerman spatial orientation and spatial visualization factors. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 32(4), 1029-1033.Google Scholar
  5. Brainard, D. H. (1997). The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 433–436.Google Scholar
  6. Brown, W. (1932). Spatial integrations in a human maze. University of California Publications in Psychology, 5, 123-134.Google Scholar
  7. Bryant, D. J., & Tversky, B. (1999). Mental representations of perspective and spatial relations from diagrams and models. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(1), 137-156.Google Scholar
  8. Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor-analytic studies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  9. Dalecki, M., Hoffmann, U., & Bock, O. (2012). Mental rotation of letters, body parts and complex scenes: Separate or common mechanisms? Human Movement Science, 31(5), 1151-1160.Google Scholar
  10. de Vega, M., & Rodrigo, M. J. (2001). Updating spatial layouts mediated by pointing and labelling under physical and imaginary rotation. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 13(3), 369-393.Google Scholar
  11. Easton, R. D., & Sholl, M. J. (1995). Object-array structure, frames of reference, and retrieval of spatial knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(2), 483-500.Google Scholar
  12. Eliot, J. (1987). Models of psychological space. New York, USA: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  13. Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175-191Google Scholar
  14. Furlanetto, T., Becchio, C., Samson, D., & Apperly, I. (2016). Altercentric interference in level 1 visual perspective taking reflects the ascription of mental states, not submentalizing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(2), 158-163.Google Scholar
  15. Goldberg, J., & Meredith, W. (1975). A longitudinal study of spatial ability. Behavior Genetics, 5(2), 127-135.Google Scholar
  16. Guilford, J. P., & Zimmerman, W. S. (1948). The Guilford-Zimmerman Aptitude Survey. Journal of Applied Psychology, 32(1), 24-34.Google Scholar
  17. Hegarty, M. (2010). Components of spatial intelligence. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 52, 265-297.Google Scholar
  18. Hegarty, M., & Waller, D. (2004). A dissociation between mental rotation and perspective-taking spatial abilities. Intelligence, 32(2), 175-191.Google Scholar
  19. Hintzman, D. L., O'Dell, C. S., & Arndt, D. R. (1981). Orientation in cognitive maps. Cognitive Psychology, 13(2), 149-206.Google Scholar
  20. JASP Team. (2018). JASP (Version 0.9) [Computer software]. https://Jasp-stats.org/
  21. Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1985). Cognitive coordinate systems: Accounts of mental rotation and individual differences in spatial ability. Psychological Review, 92(2), 137-172.Google Scholar
  22. Kosslyn, S. M. (1994). Image and brain: The resolution of the imagery debate. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  23. Kozhevnikov, M., & Hegarty, M. (2001). A dissociation between object manipulation spatial ability and spatial orientation ability. Memory & Cognition, 29(5), 745-756.Google Scholar
  24. Kyllonen, P. C., Lohman, D. F., & Woltz, D. J. (1984). Componential modeling of alternative strategies for performing spatial tasks. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76(6), 1325-1345.Google Scholar
  25. Loftus, G. R., & Masson, M. E. (1994). Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1(4), 476-490.Google Scholar
  26. Lohman, D. F. (1979). Spatial ability: A review and reanalysis of the correlational literature (No. TR-8). Stanford, CA: Stanford University California, School of Education.Google Scholar
  27. Lohman, D. F. (1988). Spatial abilities as traits, processes, and knowledge. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (Vol. 4, pp. 181-248). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  28. Lynch, K. (1960). The image of the city (Vol. 11). Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT press.Google Scholar
  29. May, M. (1996). Cognitive and embodied modes of spatial imagery. Psychologische Beitrage, 38, 418–434.Google Scholar
  30. May, M. (2004). Imaginal perspective switches in remembered environments: Transformation versus interference accounts. Cognitive Psychology, 48(2), 163-206.Google Scholar
  31. Mou, W., McNamara, T. P., Valiquette, C. M., & Rump, B. (2004). Allocentric and egocentric updating of spatial memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30(1), 142-157.Google Scholar
  32. Pelli, D. G. (1997). The videotoolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10, 437–442.Google Scholar
  33. Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1956). The child's concept of space. London: Routledge & Paul.Google Scholar
  34. Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1966/1971). Mental imagery in the child: A study of the development of imaginal representation (PA Chilton, Trans.). New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  35. Price, L., & Eliot, J. (1975). Convergent and discriminant validities of two sets of measures of spatial orientation and visualization. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 35(4), 975-977.Google Scholar
  36. Rieser, J. J. (1989). Access to knowledge of spatial structure at novel points of observation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15(6), 1157-1165.Google Scholar
  37. Roff, M. (1952). A factorial study of tests in the perceptual area (Psychometric Monograph, No. 8). Richmond, VA: Psychometric Corporation.Google Scholar
  38. Shelton, A. L., & McNamara, T. P. (1997). Multiple views of spatial memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4(1), 102-106.Google Scholar
  39. Shepard, R. N., & Cooper, L. A. (1982). Mental images and their transformations. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
  40. Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 171(3972), 701-703.Google Scholar
  41. Surtees, A., Apperly, I., & Samson, D. (2013). Similarities and differences in visual and spatial perspective-taking processes. Cognition, 129(2), 426-438.Google Scholar
  42. StatSoft, Inc. (2014). STATISTICA (Version 12) [Data analysis software system]. www.statsoft.com.
  43. Wolbers, T., & Hegarty, M. (2010). What determines our navigational abilities? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(3), 138-146.Google Scholar
  44. Wraga, M., Shephard, J. M., Church, J. A., Inati, S., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2005). Imagined rotations of self versus objects: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 43(9), 1351-1361.Google Scholar
  45. Zacks, J. M., & Michelon, P. (2005). Transformations of visuospatial images. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 4(2), 96-118.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© The Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyBen-Gurion University of the NegevBeer-ShevaIsrael

Personalised recommendations