Advertisement

The role of retrieval during study: Evidence of reminding from self-paced study time

  • Geoffrey L. McKinleyEmail author
  • Brian H. Ross
  • Aaron S. Benjamin
Article
  • 24 Downloads

Abstract

The reminding effect (Tullis, Benjamin, & Ross, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143[4], 1526–1540, 2014) describes the increase in recall of a studied word when a related word is presented later in the study list. However, because the process of reminding is thought to occur during study, measures of test performance are indirect indicators of the process of reminding and are subject to influences that arise during testing. The present research seeks evidence of reminding during encoding. In two experiments, self-paced study times were used to index the online process of reminding. In Experiment 1, pairs of repeated words, related words, and unrelated words were included in a study list. Study times were shorter for words related to prior words in the list, but only when the lag between those two words was short. Relatedness affected study time by inspiring a reduction in the threshold for termination of study for related words under massed conditions. Experiment 2 replicated the reduction in study time for related words and further showed that the study time allotted to an associate of an earlier item predicted better memory for that earlier word on a cued-recall test. In this experiment, an advantage in memory was observed for related words, and self-paced study time of one word during encoding was predictive of later memory for a related word. These results suggest a link between the action of reminding at study, as indexed by changes in the distribution of study time, and later benefits to remembering, as revealed by the reminding effect.

Keywords

Reminding Self-paced study Study time 

Notes

References

  1. Anders, R., Alario, F., & van Maanen, L. (2016). The shifted Wald distribution for response time data analysis. Psychological Methods, 21(3), 309–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Batchelder, W. H., & Riefer, D. M. (1980). Separation of storage and retrieval factors in free recall of clusterable pairs. Psychological Review, 87, 375–397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Benjamin, A. S., & Bjork, R. A. (1996). Retrieval fluency as a metacognitive index. In L. Reder (Ed.), Implicit memory and metacognition. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  4. Benjamin, A. S., Bjork, R. A., & Schwartz, B. L. (1998). The mismeasure of memory: When retrieval fluency is misleading as a metamnemonic index. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 55–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Benjamin, A. S., & Pashler, H. (2015). The value of standardized testing: A perspective from cognitive psychology. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 2(1), 13–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Benjamin, A. S., & Ross, B. H. (2010). The causes and consequences of reminding. In A. S. Benjamin (Ed.), Successful remembering and successful forgetting: A Festschrift in honor of Robert A. Bjork. New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  7. Benjamin, A. S., & Tullis, J. G. (2010). What makes distributed practice effective? Cognitive Psychology, 61(3), 228–247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Brooks, L. R., Norman, G. R., & Allen, S. W. (1991). Role of specific similarity in a medical diagnostic task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 278–287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bruce, D., & Weaver, G. E. (1973). Retroactive facilitation in short-term retention of minimally learned paired associates. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 100, 9–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Challis, B. H. (1993). Spacing effects on cued-memory tests depend on level of processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19(2), 389–396.Google Scholar
  11. Cramer, J. S. (1991). The logit model: An introduction for economists. London, UK: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
  12. Crowder, R. G. (1976). Principles of learning and memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  13. Ernst, M. D. (2004). Permutation methods: A basis for exact inference. Statistical Science, 19(4), 676–685.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Folks, J. L., & Chhikara, R. S. (1978). The inverse Gaussian distribution and its statistical application. Journal of Royal Statistical Society of Great Britain, 40, 263–289.Google Scholar
  15. Fraundorf, S. H., Watson, D. G., & Benjamin, A. S. (2015). Reduction in prosodic prominence predicts speakers' recall: implications for theories of prosody. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30(5), 606–619.Google Scholar
  16. Gick, M. L., & Holyoak, K. J. (1980). Analogical problem solving. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 306–355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Gilovich, T. (1981). Seeing the past in the present: The effect of associations to familiar events on judgments and decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 797–808.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Glanzer, M. (1969). Distance between related words in free recall: Trace of the STS. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 105–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Greene, R. L. (1989). Spacing effects in memory: Evidence for a two-process account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 371–377.Google Scholar
  20. Greeno, J. G. (1970). Conservation of information-processing capacity in paired-associate memorizing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9, 581–586.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Hintzman, D. L. (2004). Judgment of frequency vs. recognition confidence: Repetition and recursive reminding. Memory & Cognition, 32, 336–350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hintzman, D. L. (2010). How does repetition affect memory? Evidence from judgments of recency. Memory & Cognition, 38, 102–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Hintzman, D. L. (2011). Research strategy in the study of memory: Fads, fallacies, and the search for the “coordinates of truth.”. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(3), 253–271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hintzman, D. L., Asher, S. J., & Stern, L. D. (1978). Incidental retrieval and memory for coincidences. In M. M. Gruneberg, P. E. Morris, & R. N. Sykes (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory. London, UK: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  25. Hintzman, D. L., Summers, J. J., & Block, R. A. (1975). Spacing judgments as an index of study-phase retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 1, 31–40.Google Scholar
  26. Jacoby, L. L. (1974). The role of mental contiguity in memory: Registration and retrieval effects. Journal of Memory & Language, 13(5), 483–496.Google Scholar
  27. Jacoby, L. L., & Wahlheim, C. N. (2013). On the importance of looking back: The role of recursive remindings in recency judgments and cued recall. Memory &Cognition, 41, 625–637.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Jacoby L. L., Wahlheim C. N., & Yonelinas A. P. (2013). The role of detection and recollection of change in list discrimination. Memory & Cognition, 41, 638–649.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Karpicke, J. D., & Roediger, H. L. (2007). Repeated retrieval during learning is the key to long-term retention. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 151–162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Karpicke, J. D., & Roediger, H. L. (2008). The critical importance of retrieval for learning. Science, 319, 966–968.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Loftus, G. R., & Masson, M. E. J. (1994). Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 476–490.Google Scholar
  32. Luce, R. D. (1986). Response times: Their role in inferring elementary mental organization. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  33. Matzke, D., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2009). Psychological interpretation of the ex-Gaussian and shifted Wald parameters: A diffusion model analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 798–817.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Medin, D. L., & Schaffer, M. M. (1978). A context theory of classification learning. Psychological Review, 85, 207–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Nelson, D. L., McEvoy, C. L., & Schreiber, T. A. (2004). The University of South Florida word association, rhyme, and word fragment norms. Behavior Research Methods Instruments,& Computers, 36, 402–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Nyberg, L., Tulving, E., Habib, R., Nilsson, L.-G., Kapur, S., Houle, S., . . . McIntosh, A. R. (1995). Functional brain maps of retrieval mode and recovery of episode information. Neuroreport, 6, 249–252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Ratcliff, R. (1978). A theory of memory retrieval. Psychological Review, 85, 59–108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Robbins, D., & Bray, J. F. (1974). Repetition effects and retroactive facilitation: Immediate and delayed test performance. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 3, 347–349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Ross, B. H. (1984). Remindings and their effects in learning a cognitive skill. Cognitive Psychology, 16, 371–416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Ross, B. H. (1987). This is like that: The use of earlier problems and separation of similarity effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13,629–639.Google Scholar
  41. Ross, B. H., & Bradshaw, G. L. (1994). Encoding effects of remindings. Memory & Cognition, 22, 591–605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Ross, B. H., Perkins, S. J., & Tenpenny, P. L. (1990). Reminding-based category learning. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 460–492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Rundus, D. (1971). Analysis of rehearsal processes in free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 89, 63–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Shaughnessy, J. J., Zimmerman, J., & Underwood, B. J. (1972). Further evidence on the MP-DP effect in free recall learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 1–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Son, L. K., & Metcalfe, J. (2000). Metacognitive and control strategies in study-time allocation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 26, 204–221.Google Scholar
  46. Tullis, M., Benjamin, A. S., & Ross, B. H. (2014). The reminding effect: Presentation of associates enhances memory for related words in a list. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(4), 1526–1540.Google Scholar
  47. Tullis, J. G., Braverman, M., Ross, B. H., & Benjamin, A. S. (2014). Remindings influence the interpretation of ambiguous stimuli. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21, 107–113.Google Scholar
  48. Tullis, J. G., Finley, J. R., & Benjamin, A. S. (2013). Metacognition of the testing effect: Guiding learners to predict the benefits of retrieval. Memory & Cognition, 41, 429–442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Tzeng, O. L., & Cotton, B. (1980). A study-phase retrieval model of temporal coding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 705–716.Google Scholar
  50. Underwood, B. J. (1969). Some correlates of item repetition in free-recall learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 83–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Underwood, B. J. (1970). A breakdown of total-time law in free-recall learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9, 573–580.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Van Zandt, T. (2000). How to fit a response time distribution. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7(3), 424–465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Van Zandt, T. (2002). Analysis of response time distributions. In J. T. Wixted (Ed.), Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology (3rd ed., pp. 461–516). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  54. Wahlheim, C. N., & Jacoby, L. L. (2013). Remembering change: The critical role of recursive remindings in proactive effects of memory. Memory & Cognition, 41, 115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Waugh, N. C. (1970). On the effective duration of a repeated word. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9, 587–595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Winograd, E., & Soloway, R. M. (1985). Reminding as a basis for temporal judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 262–271.Google Scholar
  57. Zimmerman, J. (1975). Free recall after self-paced study: A test of the attention explanation of the spacing effect. The American Journal of Psychology, 88, 277–291.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© The Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.University of Illinois at Urbana-ChampaignChampaignUSA
  2. 2.School of PsychologyGeorgia Institute of TechnologyAtlantaUSA
  3. 3.Minerva Schools at KGISan FranciscoUSA

Personalised recommendations