Advertisement

Visual working memory load does not eliminate visuomotor repetition effects

  • Jason RajsicEmail author
  • Matthew D. Hilchey
  • Geoffrey F. Woodman
  • Jay Pratt
Article
  • 19 Downloads

Abstract

When we respond to a stimulus, our ability to quickly execute this response depends on how combinations of stimulus and response features match to previous combinations of stimulus and response features. Some kind of memory representations must be underlying these visuomotor repetition effects. In this paper, we tested the hypothesis that visual working memory stores the stimulus information that gives rise to these effects. Participants discriminated the colors of successive stimuli while holding either three locations or colors in visual working memory. If visual working memory maintains the information about a previous event that leads to visuomotor repetition effects, then occupying working memory with colors or locations should selectively disrupt color–response and location–response repetition effects. The results of two experiments showed that neither color nor spatial memory load eliminated visuomotor repetition effects. Since working memory load did not disrupt repetition effects, it is unlikely that visual working memory resources are used to store the information that underlies visuomotor repetitions effects. Instead, these results are consistent with the view that visuomotor repetition effects stem from automatic long-term memory retrieval, but can also be accommodated by supposing separate buffers for visual working memory and response selection.

Keywords

Visual working memory Repetition effects Memory: visual working and short-term memory 

Notes

Acknowledgements

Funding for this project was provided by a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada grant to J.P. (194537), National Institutes of Health grants to G.F.W. (R01-EY025275, R01-EY019882, R01-MH110378, and P30-EY08126), a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada Post-Graduate Scholarship to J.R., and a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada Post-doctoral scholarship to M.D.H.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Open practices statement

The data for the experiments reported here are available at https://osf.io/d7tr6/, and none of the experiments were preregistered.

Supplementary material

13414_2019_1839_MOESM1_ESM.doc (120 kb)
ESM 1 (DOC 120 kb)

References

  1. Ahn, J. W., Patel, T. N., Buetti, S., & Lleras, A. (2017). Exploring the contributions of spatial and non-spatial working memory to priming of pop-out. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 79(4), 1012–1026.Google Scholar
  2. Bertelson, P. (1965). Serial choice reaction-time as a function of response versus signal-and-response repetition. Nature, 206(4980), 217-218.Google Scholar
  3. Bundesen, C. (1990). A theory of visual attention. Psychological Review, 97(4), 523–547.Google Scholar
  4. Carlisle, N. B. & Kristjánsson, À. (2018). How visual working memory contents influence priming of visual attention. Psychological Research, 82(5), 1-7.Google Scholar
  5. Castel, A. D., Pratt, J., & Craik, F. I. M. (2003). The role of spatial working memory in inhibition of return: Evidence from divided attention tasks. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 65(6), 970–981.Google Scholar
  6. Colzato, L. S., Raffone, A., & Hommel, B. (2006). What do we learn from binding features? Evidence for multilevel feature integration. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Human Perception and Performance, 32(3), 705–716.Google Scholar
  7. Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 24(1), 87–114.Google Scholar
  8. Gade, M., Druey, M. D., Souza, A. S., & Oberauer, K. (2014). Interference within and between declarative and procedural representations in working memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 76, 174–194.Google Scholar
  9. Hartshorne, J. K. (2008). Visual working memory capacity and proactive interference. PLOS ONE, 3(7), e2716.Google Scholar
  10. Henson, R. N., Eckstein, D., Waszak, F., Frings, C., & Horner, A. J. (2014). Stimulus-response bindings in priming. Trends in Cognitive Science, 18(7), 376-384.Google Scholar
  11. Hilchey, M. D., Antinucci, V., Lamy, D., & Pratt, J. (in press). Is attention really biased toward the last target location in visual search? Attention, response rules, distractors, and eye movements. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.Google Scholar
  12. Hilchey, M. D., Rajsic, J., Huffman, G., Klein, R. M., & Pratt, J. (2018). Dissociating orienting biases from integration effects with eye movements. Psychological Science, 29(3), 328-339.Google Scholar
  13. Hilchey, M. D., Rajsic, J., Huffman, G., & Pratt, J. (2017a). Intervening response events between identification targets do not always turn repetition benefits into repetition costs. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 1–13.Advance online publication.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-016-1262-9
  14. Hilchey, M. D., Rajsic, J., Huffman, G., & Pratt, J. (2017b). Response-mediated spatial priming despite perfectly valid target location cues and intervening response events. Visual Cognition, 1–15. Advance online publication.  https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2017.1349230
  15. Hommel, B. (1998). Event files: Evidence for automatic integration of stimulus-response episodes. Visual Cognition, 5(1-2), 183-216.Google Scholar
  16. Hommel, B. (2004). Event files: Feature binding in and across perception and action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(11), 494–500.Google Scholar
  17. Hommel, B. (2005). How much attention does an event file need? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(5), 1067–1082.Google Scholar
  18. Hommel, B. (2019). Theory of event coding (TEC) V2.0: Representing and controlling perception and action. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 1–16.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-019-01779-4
  19. Hommel, B., & Colzato, L. S. (2004). Visual attention and the temporal dynamics of feature integration. Visual Cognition, 11(4), 483–521.Google Scholar
  20. Hommel, B. & Colzato, L. S. (2009). When an object is more than the binding of its features: Evidence for two mechanisms of feature integration. Visual Cognition, 17(1/2), 120–140.Google Scholar
  21. Hommel, B., Memelink, J., Zmigrod, S., & Colzato, L. S. (2014). Attentional control of the creation and retrieval of stimulus-response bindings. Psychological Research, 78(4), 520–538.Google Scholar
  22. Hommel, B., Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K. P. L. (2004). A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task. Psychological Research, 68(1), 1–17.Google Scholar
  23. Huffman, G., Hilchey, M. D., & Pratt, J. (2018). Feature integration in basic detection and localization tasks: Insights from the attentional orienting literature. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 80(6), 1333–1341.Google Scholar
  24. JASP Team. (2018). JASP (Version 0.9) [Computer software]. Retrieved from https://jasp-stats.org/2018/06/20/introducing-jasp-0-9/
  25. Jiang, Y., Olson, I. R., & Chun, M. M. (2000). Organization of visual short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, memory, & cognition, 26(3), 683–702.Google Scholar
  26. Kahneman, D., Treisman, A., & Gibbs, B. J. (1992). The reviewing of object files: Object-specific integration of information. Cognitive Psychology, 24(2), 175–219.Google Scholar
  27. Keele, S. W., & Boies, S. J. (1973). Processing demands of sequential information. Memory & Cognition, 1(1), 85–90.Google Scholar
  28. Keizer, A. W., Hommel, B., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2015). Consciousness is not necessary for visual feature binding. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22, 453-460.Google Scholar
  29. Kleinsorge, T. (1999). Response repetition benefits and costs. Acta Psychologica, 103(3), 295–310.Google Scholar
  30. Kleiner, M., Brainard, D. H., & Pelli, D. G. (2007). What is new in Psychophysics Toolbox. Perception, 36.Google Scholar
  31. Kristjánsson, À., Saevarsson, S., & Driver, J. (2013). The boundary conditions of priming of visual search: From passive viewing through task-relevant working memory load. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(3), 514–521.Google Scholar
  32. Lee, H., Mozer, M. C., & Vecera, S. P. (2009). Mechanisms of priming of pop-out: Stored representations or feature-gain modulations? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71(5), 1059–1071.Google Scholar
  33. Lin, P. H., & Luck, S. J. (2012). Proactive interference does not meaningfully distort visual working memory capacity estimates in the canonical change detection task. Frontiers in psychology, 3, 42.Google Scholar
  34. Logan, G. D. (1979). On the use of a concurrent memory load to measure attention and automaticity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5(2), 189.Google Scholar
  35. Logan, G. D. (1990). Repetition priming and automaticity: Common underlying mechanisms? Cognitive Psychology, 22, 1–35.Google Scholar
  36. Logie, R. H., Brockmole, J. R., & Vandenbroucke, A. R. (2009). Bound feature combinations in visual short-term memory are fragile but influence long-term learning. Visual Cognition, 17(1/2), 160–179.Google Scholar
  37. Luck, S. J., & Vogel, E. K. (1997). The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions. Nature, 390(6657), 279–281.Google Scholar
  38. Makovski, T., & Jiang, Y. V. (2008). Proactive interference from items previously stored in visual working memory. Memory & Cognition, 36(1), 43–52.Google Scholar
  39. Maljkovic, V., & Nakayama, K. (2000). Priming of popout: III. A short-term implicit memory system beneficial for rapid target selection. Visual cognition, 7(5), 571–595.Google Scholar
  40. Maljkovic, V., & Nakayama, K. (1994). Priming of pop-out: I. Role of features. Memory & Cognition, 22(6), 657–672.Google Scholar
  41. Memelink, J., & Hommel, B. (2013). Intentional weighting: A basic principle in cognitive control. Psychological Research, 77(3), 249–259.Google Scholar
  42. Moeller, B., & Frings, C. (2017). Dissociation of binding and learning processes. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 79(8), 2590–2605. doi: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-017-1393-7 Google Scholar
  43. Oberauer, K. (2009). Design for a working memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 51, 45–100.Google Scholar
  44. Oberauer, K., Awh, E., & Sutterer, D. W. (2017). The role of long-term memory in a test of visual working memory: Proactive facilitation but no proactive interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43(1), 1–22.Google Scholar
  45. Pashler, H., & Baylis, G. (1991). Procedural learning: II. Intertrial repetition effects in speeded-choice tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17(1), 33–48.Google Scholar
  46. Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. Attention and Performance X: Control of Language Processes, 32, 531–556.Google Scholar
  47. Pösse, B., Waszak, F., & Hommel, B. (2006). Do stimulus-response bindings survive a task switch? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 18(4), 640–651.Google Scholar
  48. Rajsic, J., Bi, Y., & Wilson, D. E. (2014). Long-term facilitation of return: A response-retrieval effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(2), 418–424.Google Scholar
  49. Roper, Z. J. J., & Vecera, S. P. (2014). Visual short-term memory load strengthens selective attention. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(2), 549–556.Google Scholar
  50. Rothermund, K., Wentura, D., & De Houwer, J. (2005). Retrieval of incidental stimulus–response associations as a source of negative priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(3), 482–495.Google Scholar
  51. Rouder, J. N., Morey, R. D., Morey, C. C., & Cowan, N. (2011). How to measure working memory capacity in the change detection paradigm. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(2), 324–330.Google Scholar
  52. Schmidt, J. R., De Houwer, J., & Rothermund, K. (2016). The parallel episodic processing (PEP) model 2.0: A single computational model of stimulus-response binding, contingency learning, power curves, and mixing costs. Cognitive Psychology, 91, 82–108.Google Scholar
  53. Schneider, D. W. & Anderson, J. R. (2011). A memory-based model of Hick’s law. Cognitive Psychology, 62(3), 193–222.Google Scholar
  54. Souza, A. S., Oberauer, K., Gade, M., & Druey, M. D. (2012). Processing of representations in declarative and procedural working memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(5), 1006–1033.Google Scholar
  55. Waszak, F., Hommel, B., & Allport, A. (2003). Task-switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic stimulus-task bindings in task-shift costs. Cognitive Psychology, 46(4), 361–413.Google Scholar
  56. Wilson, D. E., Castel, A. D., & Pratt, J. (2006). Long-term inhibition of return for spatial locations: Evidence for a memory retrieval account. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(12), 2135–2147.Google Scholar
  57. Woodman, G. F. & Luck, S. J. (2004). Visual search is slowed when visuospatial working memory is occupied. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(2), 269–274.Google Scholar
  58. Woodman, G. F., & Vogel, E. K. (2008). Selective storage and maintenance of an object’s features in visual working memory. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 15(1), 223–229.Google Scholar
  59. Woodman, G. F., Vogel, E. K., & Luck (2012). Flexibility in visual working memory: Accurate change detection in the face of irrelevant variations in position. Visual Cognition, 20(1), 1–28.Google Scholar
  60. Yamaguchi, M., & Proctor, R. W. (2006). Stimulus-response compatibility with pure and mixed mappings in a flight task environment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 12(4), 207–222.Google Scholar
  61. Zhang, Y., & Zhang, M. (2011). Spatial working memory load impairs manual but not saccadic inhibition of return. Vision Research, 51(1), 147–153.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© The Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  • Jason Rajsic
    • 1
    Email author
  • Matthew D. Hilchey
    • 2
  • Geoffrey F. Woodman
    • 1
  • Jay Pratt
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyVanderbilt UniversityNashvilleUSA
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversity of TorontoTorontoUSA

Personalised recommendations