Advertisement

Crossing event boundaries changes prospective perceptions of temporal length and proximity

  • Ashley S. BangertEmail author
  • Christopher A. Kurby
  • Allyson S. Hughes
  • Omar Carrasco
Article
  • 4 Downloads

Abstract

We conducted two experiments to investigate how crossing a single naturalistic event boundary impacted two different types of temporal estimation involving the same target duration – one where participants directly compared marked temporal durations and another where they judged the temporal proximity of stimuli. In Experiment 1, participants judged whether time intervals presented during movies of everyday events were shorter or longer than a previously encoded 5-s reference interval. We examined how the presence of a transition between events (event boundary) in the movie influenced people’s judgments about the length of the comparison interval. Comparison intervals presented during a portion of the movie containing an event boundary were judged as shorter than the reference interval more often than comparison intervals that contained no boundary. Working-memory updating at the event boundary may have directed attention away from the concurrent timing task. In Experiment 2, participants judged whether the second of three tones presented during everyday movies was closer to the first or the third tone presented. Tones separated by an event boundary were judged as farther apart than tones contained within the same event. When judging temporal proximity, attention directed to processing information at an event boundary between two stimuli may disrupt the formation of temporal associations between those stimuli. Overall, these results demonstrate that crossing a single event boundary can impact people’s prospective perceptions of the temporal characteristics of their experience and suggest that the episodic memory updating that occurs during an event boundary both captures timing-relevant attentional resources and plays a role in the temporal binding of information.

Keywords

Event cognition Time perception Temporal proximity Event segmentation theory Attention 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Christian Ruiz for his assistance with participant testing. The authors report no conflicts of interest.

Open Practices Statement

None of the data or materials for the experiments reported here is available, and none of the experiments was preregistered.

References

  1. Bailey, H. R., Kurby, C. A., Sargent, J. Q., & Zacks, J. M. (2017). Attentional focus affects how events are segmented and updated in narrative reading. Memory & Cognition, 45, 940-955.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bangert, A. S., Kurby, C. A. & Zacks, J. M. (2019). The influence of everyday events on prospective timing “in the moment”. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(2), 677-684. http://0-dx.doi.org.lib.utep.edu/10.3758/s13423-018-1526-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Ben-Yakov, A., Eshel, N., & Dudai, Y. (2013). Hippocampal immediate poststimulus activity in the encoding of consecutive naturalistic episodes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(4), 1255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bisby, J. A. & Burgess, N. (2014). Negative affect impairs associative memory but not item memory. Learning & Memory, 21, 21-27. doi:  https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.032409.113 Google Scholar
  5. Bisby, J. A. & Burgess, N. (2017). Differential effects of negative emotion on memory for items and associations, and their relationship to intrusive imagery. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 17, 124-132. doi:  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.07.012 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Block, R. A. & Gruber, R. P. (2014). Time perception, attention and memory: A selective review. Acta Psychologica, 149, 123-133. doi:  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.11.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Block, R. A., & Reed, M. A. (1978). Remembered duration: Evidence for a contextual-change hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory, 4, 656-665.Google Scholar
  8. Block, R. A. & Zakay, D. (1997). Prospective and retrospective duration judgments: A meta-analytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 184-197. doi:  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03209393 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Boltz, M. G. (1995). Effects of event structure on retrospective duration judgments. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 1080-1096. doi: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03205466 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Brown, S. W. (1997). Attentional resources in timing: Interference effects in concurrent temporal and nontemporal working memory tasks. Perception & Psychophysics, 59(7), 1118-1140. http://0-dx.doi.org.lib.utep.edu/10.3758/BF03205526 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Brown, S. W., Collier, S. A. & Night, J. C. (2013). Timing and executive resources: Dual-task interference patterns between temporal production and shifting, updating and inhibition tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human, Perception and Performance, 39(4), 947-963.Google Scholar
  12. Brown, S. W., Johnson, T. M., Sohl, M. E. & Dumas, M. K. (2015). Executive attentional resources in timing: Effects of inhibitory control and cognitive aging. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human, Perception and Performance, 41(4), 1063-1083.Google Scholar
  13. Brown, S. W. & Perreault, S. T. (2017). Relation between temporal perception and inhibitory control in a Go/No-Go task. Acta Psychologica, 173, 87-93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Cohen, N. J., Poldrack, R. A. & Eichenbaum, H. (1997). Memory for items and memory for relations in the procedural/declarative framework. Memory, 5(1-2), 131-178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Davachi, L. (2006). Item, context and relational episodic encoding in humans. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16, 693-700.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. DuBrow, S., & Davachi, L. (2013). The influence of context boundaries on memory for the sequential order of events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 1277–1286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. DuBrow, S., & Davachi, L. (2014). Temporal memory is shaped by encoding stability and intervening item reactivation. The Journal of Neuroscience, 34(42), 13998-14005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. DuBrow, S., & Davachi, L. (2016). Temporal binding within and across events. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 134, 107-114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Ezzyat, Y. & Davachi, L. (2011). What constitutes an episode in episodic memory? Psychological Science, 22(2), 243-252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Ezzyat, Y. & Davachi, L. (2014). Similarity breeds proximity: Pattern similarity within and across contexts is related to later mnemonic judgments of temporal proximity. Neuron, 81, 1179-1189. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2014.01.042 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Faber, M., & Gennari, S. P. (2015). In search of lost time: Reconstructing the unfolding of events from memory. Cognition, 143, 193–202. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.06.014 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Faber, M. & Gennari, S. P. (2017). Effects of learned episodic event structure on prospective duration judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 43(8), 1203-1214. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000378 Google Scholar
  23. Grondin, S. (2010). Timing and time perception. A review of recent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoretical directions. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 72(3), 561-582.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Heusser, A. C., Ezzyat, Y., Shiff, I. & Davachi, L. (2018). Perceptual boundaries cause mnemonic trade-offs between local boundary processing and across-trial associative binding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 44(7), 1075-1090.Google Scholar
  25. Kersten, A. W., Earles, J. L., Curtayne, E. S. & Lane, J. C. (2008). Adult age differences in binding actors and actions in memory for events. Memory & Cognition, 36(1), 119-131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Kurby, C. A. & Zacks, J. M. (2008). Segmentation in the perception and memory of events. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(2), 72-79. doi:  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.11.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Kurby, C. A. & Zacks, J. M. (2011). Age differences in the perception of hierarchical structure in events. Memory & Cognition, 39, 75-91. doi:  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-010-0027-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Lejeune, H. & Wearden, J. H. (2009). Vierordt’s The Experimental Study of the Time Sense (1868) and its legacy. The European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 21(6), 941 – 960. doi:  https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440802453006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Liverence, B. M & Scholl, B. J. (2012). Discrete events as units of perceived time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 38(2), 549-554.Google Scholar
  30. Lositsky, O., Chen, J., Toker, D., Honey, C. J., Shvartsman, M., Poppenk, J. L., Hasson, U. & Norman, K.A. (2016). Neural pattern change during encoding of a narrative predicts retrospective duration estimates. eLife, 2016; 5: e16070. doi:  https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16070 Google Scholar
  31. Macar, F., Grondin, S., & Casini, L. (1994). Controlled attention sharing influences time estimation. Memory & Cognition, 22, 673–686. doi: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03209252 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Newtson, D. (1976). Foundations of attribution: The perception of ongoing behavior. In J. H. Harvey, W. J. Ickes, & R. F. Kidd (Eds.), New directions in attribution research (pp. 223–248). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  33. Newtson, D., & Engquist, G. (1976). The perceptual organization of ongoing behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 12, 436–450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Old, S. R. & Naveh-Benjamin, M. (2008). Differential effects of age on item and associative measures of memory: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 23 (1), 104-118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Pӧppel, E. (2004). Lost in time: a historical frame, elementary processing units and the 3-second window. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 64, 295-301.Google Scholar
  36. Poynter, W. D. (1983). Duration judgment and the segmentation of experience. Memory & Cognition, 11, 77-82.Google Scholar
  37. Poynter, W. D. & Homa, D. (1983). Duration judgment and the experience of change. Perception & Psychophysics, 33(6), 548-560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Predebon, J. (1996). The relationship between the number of presented stimuli and prospective duration estimates: The effect of concurrent task activity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3(3), 376-379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Radvansky, G. A., & Copeland, D. E. (2006). Walking through doorways causes forgetting: Situation models and experienced space. Memory & Cognition, 34, 1150-1156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Radvansky, G. A., Krawietz, S. A. & Tamplin, A. K. (2011). Walking through doorways causes forgetting: Further explorations. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(8), 1632-1645. doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2011.571267 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Radvansky, G. A., Tamplin, A. K., & Krawietz, S. A. (2010). Walking through doorways causes forgetting: Environmental integration. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 900–904.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Tse, P. U., Intriligator, P., Rivest, J. & Cavanagh, P. (2004). Attention and the subjective expansion of time. Perception & Psychophysics, 66(7), 1171-1189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Waldum, E. R. & Sahakyan, L. (2013). A role for memory in prospective timing informs timing in prospective memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(3), 809-826. doi:  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030113 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Zacks, J. M., Kurby, C. A., Eisenberg, M. L. & Haroutunian, N. (2011). Prediction error associated with the perceptual segmentation of naturalistic events. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(12), 4057-4066.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Zacks, J. M., Speer, N. K. & Reynolds, J. R. (2009). Segmentation in reading and film comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138(2), 307-327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Zacks, J. M., Speer, N. K., Swallow, K. M., Braver, T. S. & Reynolds, J. R. (2007). Event perception: A mind-brain perspective. Psychological Bulletin, 133(2), 273-293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Zacks, J. M., Speer, N. K., Vettel, J. M. & Jacoby, L. L. (2006). Event understanding and memory in healthy aging and dementia of the Alzheimer type. Psychology and Aging, 21(3), 466-482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Zakay, D. & Block, R. A. (1995). An attentional-gate model of prospective time estimation. In M. Richelle, V. D. Keyser, G. d'Ydewalle, & A. Vandierendonck (Eds.), Time and the dynamic control of behavior (pp. 167–178). Liège, Belgium: Universite Liège.Google Scholar
  49. Zauberman, G., Levav, J., Diehl, K. & Bhargrave, R. (2010). 1995 Feels So Close Yet So Far: The Effect of Event Markers on Subjective Feelings of Elapsed Time. Psychological Science, 21(1), 133-139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© The Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  • Ashley S. Bangert
    • 1
    Email author
  • Christopher A. Kurby
    • 2
  • Allyson S. Hughes
    • 1
    • 3
  • Omar Carrasco
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyThe University of Texas at El PasoEl PasoUSA
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyGrand Valley State UniversityAllendaleUSA
  3. 3.T1DExchangeBostonUSA

Personalised recommendations