Advertisement

Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics

, Volume 81, Issue 8, pp 2732–2744 | Cite as

Probability-driven and stimulus-driven orienting of attention to time and sensory modality

  • Melisa MencelogluEmail author
  • Marcia Grabowecky
  • Satoru Suzuki
Article
  • 126 Downloads

Abstract

The timing and the sensory modality of behaviorally relevant events often vary predictably, so that it is beneficial to adapt the sensory system to their statistical regularities. Indeed, statistical information about target timing and/or sensory modality modulates behavioral responses—called expectation effects. Responses are also facilitated by short-term repetitions of target timing and/or sensory modality—called priming effects. We examined how the expectation and priming effects on target timing (short vs. long cue-to-target interval) and target modality (auditory vs. visual) interacted. Temporal expectation was manipulated across blocks, while modality expectation was manipulated across participants. Responses were faster when targets were presented at the expected timing and/or in the expected modality in an additive manner, suggesting that temporal and modality expectation operate relatively independently. Similarly, responses were faster when the timing and/or modality of targets was repeated across trials in an additive manner, suggesting that temporal and modality priming operate relatively independently. Importantly, the interactions between expectation and priming were domain specific. In the temporal domain, temporal-expectation effects were observed only when temporal-priming effects were absent. In the modality domain, modality-priming effects predominated for auditory targets whereas modality-expectation effects predominated for visual targets. Thus, the interactions between probability-driven expectation and stimulus-driven priming processes appear to be controlled separately for the mechanisms that direct attention to specific temporal intervals and for the mechanisms that direct attention to specific sensory modalities. These results may suggest that the sensory system concurrently optimizes attentional priorities within temporal and sensory-modality domains.

Keywords

Temporal processing Attention: Selective 

Notes

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by a National Institutes of Health grant (T32 NS047987).

Compliance with ethical standards

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants involved in the study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Open practices statement

None of the data or materials for the experiment reported here are available online, but the data and materials can be provided upon request. The experiment was not preregistered.

Supplementary material

13414_2019_1798_MOESM1_ESM.docx (650 kb)
ESM 1 (DOCX 650 kb)

References

  1. Brainard, D. H. (1997). The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 433–436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Capizzi, M., Correa, Á., Wojtowicz, A., & Rafal, R. D. (2015). Foreperiod priming in temporal preparation: Testing current models of sequential effects. Cognition, 134, 39–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Colavita, F. B. (1974). Human sensory dominance. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 16(2), 409–412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Correa, A., Lupiáñez, J., Milliken, B., & Tudela, P. (2004). Endogenous temporal orienting of attention in detection and discrimination tasks. Perception & Psychophysics, 66, 264–278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Coull, J. T. (2009). Neural substrates of mounting temporal expectation. PLOS Biology, 7(8), e1000166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Coull, J. T., & Nobre, A. C. (1998). Where and when to pay attention: The neural systems for directing attention to spatial locations and to time intervals as revealed by both PET and fMRI. The Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 7426–7435.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Coull, J. T. & Nobre, A. C. (2008). Dissociating explicit timing from temporal expectation with fMRI. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 18, 137–144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Doherty, J. R., Rao, A., Mesulam, M. M., & Nobre, A. C. (2005). Synergistic effect of combined temporal and spatial expectations on visual attention. Journal of Neuroscience, 25(36), 8259–8266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Foxe, J. J., Simpson, G. V., Ahlfors, S. P., & Saron, C. D. (2005). Biasing the brain’s attentional set: I. Cue driven deployments of intersensory selective attention. Experimental Brain Research, 166(3/4), 370–392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Foxe, J. J., & Snyder, A. C. (2011). The role of alpha-band brain oscillations as a sensory suppression mechanism during selective attention. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Grill-Spector, K., Henson, R., & Martin, A. (2006). Repetition and the brain: Neural models of stimulus-specific effects. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(1), 14–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Keil, J., Pomper, U., Feuerbach, N., & Senkowski, D. (2017). Temporal orienting precedes intersensory attention and has opposing effects on early evoked brain activity. NeuroImage, 148, 230–239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Keil, J., Pomper, U., & Senkowski, D. (2016). Distinct patterns of local oscillatory activity and functional connectivity underlie intersensory attention and temporal prediction. Cortex, 74, 277–288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Kleiner, M., Brainard, D., & Pelli, D. (2007). What’s new in Psychtoolbox-3? Perception, 36, ECVP abstract supplement.Google Scholar
  15. Lange, K., Krämer, U. R., & Röder, B. (2006). Attending points in time and space. Experimental Brain Research, 173, 130–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Lange, K., & Röder, B. (2006). Orienting attention to points in time improves stimulus processing both within and across modalities. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(5), 715–729.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Lloyd, D. M., Merat, N., McGlone, F., & Spence, C. (2003). Cross-modal links between audition and touch in covert endogenous spatial attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 65(6), 901–924.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Los, S. A. (2010). Foreperiod and sequential effects: Theory and data. In A. C. Nobre & J. T. Coull (Eds.), Attention and time (pp. 289–302). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Luck, S. J., Woodman, G. F., & Vogel, E. K. (2000). Event-related potential studies of attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(11), 432–440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Lukas, S., Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2010). Switching attention between modalities: Further evidence for visual dominance. Psychological Research, 74(3), 255–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Lukas, S., Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2014). Crossmodal attention switching: Auditory dominance in temporal discrimination tasks. Acta Psychologica, 153, 139–146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Menceloglu, M., Grabowecky, M., & Suzuki, S. (2017). Temporal expectation weights visual signals over auditory signals. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(2), 416–422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Miniussi, C., Wilding, E. L., Coull, J. T., & Nobre, A. C. (1999). Orienting attention in time. Brain, 122(8), 1507–1518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Morey, R. D. (2008). Confidence intervals from normalized data: A correction to Cousineau (2005). Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 4, 61–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Mühlberg, S., Oriolo, G., & Soto-Faraco, S. (2014). Cross-modal decoupling in temporal attention. European Journal of Neuroscience, 39(12), 2089–2097.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Mühlberg, S., & Soto-Faraco, S. (2018). Cross-modal decoupling in temporal attention between audition and touch. Psychological Research, 1–14. Advance online publication.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1023-6
  27. Nobre, A. C., Correa, A., & Coull, J. T. (2007). The hazards of time. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 17(4), 465–470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Nobre, A. C., & van Ede, F. (2018). Anticipated moments: Temporal structure in attention. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 19(1), 34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Pelli, D. G. (1997) The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10, 437–442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Pomper, U., Keil, J., Foxe, J. J., & Senkowski, D. (2015). Intersensory selective attention and temporal orienting operate in parallel and are instantiated in spatially distinct sensory and motor cortices. Human Brain Mapping, 36(8), 3246–3259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32(1), 3–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Posner, M. I., Nissen, M. J., & Klein, R. M. (1976). Visual dominance: An information-processing account of its origins and significance. Psychological Review, 83(2), 157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Ortega, L., Guzman-Martinez, E., Grabowecky, M., & Suzuki, S. (2014). Audition dominates vision in duration perception irrespective of salience, attention, and temporal discriminability. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76, 1485–1502.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Sinnett, S., Spence, C., & Soto-Faraco, S. (2007). Visual dominance and attention: The Colavita effect revisited. Perception & Psychophysics, 69(5), 673–686.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Soto-Faraco, S., & Spence, C. (2002). Modality-specific auditory and visual temporal processing deficits. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55(1), 23–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Spence, C., & Driver, J. (1996). Audiovisual links in endogenous covert spatial attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22(4), 1005.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Spence, C., & Driver, J. (1997). On measuring selective attention to an expected sensory modality. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 59(3), 389–403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Spence, C., Nicholls, M. E., & Driver, J. (2001). The cost of expecting events in the wrong sensory modality. Perception & Psychophysics, 63(2), 330–336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Steinborn, M. B., Rolke, B., Bratzke, D., & Ulrich, R. (2008). Sequential effects within a short foreperiod context: Evidence for the conditioning account of temporal preparation. Acta Psychologica, 129(2), 297–307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Talsma, D., & Kok, A. (2002). Intermodal spatial attention differs between vision and audition: An event-related potential analysis. Psychophysiology, 39(6), 689–706.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Woodrow, H. (1914). The measurement of attention. The Psychological Monographs, 17(5).Google Scholar

Copyright information

© The Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyNorthwestern UniversityEvanstonUSA
  2. 2.Interdepartmental Neuroscience ProgramNorthwestern UniversityEvanstonUSA

Personalised recommendations