Advertisement

Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics

, Volume 81, Issue 1, pp 205–216 | Cite as

The emotional attentional blink is robust to divided attention

  • Jonathan M. KeefeEmail author
  • Jocelyn L. Sy
  • Frank Tong
  • David H. Zald
Article

Abstract

The emotional attentional blink (EAB) refers to a temporary impairment in the ability to identify a target when it is preceded by an emotional distractor. It is thought to occur because the emotional salience of the distractor exogenously captures attention for a brief duration, rendering the target unattended and preventing it from reaching awareness. Here we tested the extent to which the EAB can be attenuated by inducing a diffuse top-down attentional state, which has been shown to improve target identification in an analogous attentional phenomenon, the attentional blink. Rapid sequences of landscape images were presented centrally, and participants reported the orientation of a ± 90° rotation of a landscape target. To induce a diffuse state of attention, participants were given a secondary task of monitoring for the appearance of a colored dot in the periphery. We found that emotional distractors impaired target recognition performance to comparable extents, regardless of whether or not participants concurrently performed the peripheral-monitoring task. Moreover, we found that performance of the secondary task led to an impaired ability to ignore neutral distractors. Subjective ratings of target vividness mirrored the behavioral accuracy, with frequent reports of intermediate levels of vividness suggesting that the EAB might impair target visibility in a graded manner. Our results demonstrate that the EAB is robust to manipulations of top-down attention, suggesting that the temporary capture of attention by emotionally salient stimuli involves processes that are distinct from those that produce the attentional blink.

Keywords

Attentional blink Divided attention Inattention Attentional capture 

References

  1. Akyürek, E. G., & Hommel, B. (2005). Short-term memory and the attentional blink: Capacity versus content. Memory & Cognition, 33, 654–663.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195332 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Arend, I., Johnston, S., & Shapiro, K. (2006). Task-irrelevant visual motion and flicker attenuate the attentional blink. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 600–607.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Asplund, C. L., Fougnie, D., Zughni, S., Martin, J. W., & Marois, R. (2014). The attentional blink reveals the probabilistic nature of discrete conscious perception. Psychological Science, 25, 824–831.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Asplund, C. L., Todd, J. J., Snyder A. P., Gilbert, C. M., & Marois, R. (2010). Surprise-induced blindness: A stimulus-driven attentional limit to conscious perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1372–1381.Google Scholar
  5. Brainard, D. H. (1997). The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vision 10, 433–436.  https://doi.org/10.1163/156856897X00357 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Burnham, B. R. (2010). Cognitive load modulates attentional capture by color singletons during effortful visual search. Acta Psychologica, 135, 50–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Chun, M. M., & Potter, M. C. (1995). A two-stage model for multiple target detection in rapid serial visual presentation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 109–127.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.21.1.109 Google Scholar
  8. Ciesielski, B. G., Armstrong, T., Zald, D. H., & Olatunji, B. O. (2010). Emotion modulation of visual attention: Categorical and temporal characteristics. PLoS ONE, 5, e13860.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0013860 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. de Fockert, J. W., & Bremner, A. J. (2011). Release of inattentional blindness by high working memory load: Elucidating the relationship between working memory and selective attention. Cognition, 121, 400–408.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.08.016 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. de Fockert, J. W., Rees, G., Frith, C. D., & Lavie, N. (2001). The role of working memory in visual selective attention. Science, 291, 1803–1806.  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1056496 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Erthal, F. S., De Oliveira, L., Mocaiber, I., Pereira, M. G., Machado-Pinheiro, W., Volchan, E., & Pessoa, L. (2005). Load-dependent modulation of affective picture processing. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 5, 388–395.  https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.5.4.388 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Evans, K. K., & Treisman, A. (2005) Natural cross-modal mappings between visual and auditory features. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 1476–1492.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.31.6.1476 Google Scholar
  13. Giesbrecht, B., & Di Lollo, V. (1998). Beyond the attentional blink: Visual masking by object substitution. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1454–1466.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.24.5.1454 Google Scholar
  14. Holm, S. (1979). A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics, 6, 65–70. Retrieved from www.jstor.org/stable/4615733 Google Scholar
  15. Kelley, T. A., & Lavie, N. (2010). Working memory load modulates distractor competition in primary visual cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 21, 659–665.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Kennedy, B. L., Rawding, J., Most, S. B., & Hoffman, J. E. (2014). Emotion-induced blindness reflects competition at early and late processing stages: An ERP study. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 14, 1485–1498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Kim, S.-Y., Kim, M.-S., & Chun, M. M. (2005). Concurrent working memory load can reduce distraction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102, 16524–16529.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0505454102 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2008). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual (Technical Report No. A-8). Gainesville, FL: University of Florida, NIMH Center for Research in Psychophysiology.Google Scholar
  19. Langton, S. R. H., Law, A. S., Burton, A. M., & Schweinberger, S. R. (2008). Attention capture by faces. Cognition, 107, 330–342.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.07.012 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Lavie, N., & de Fockert, J. (2005). The role of working memory in attentional capture. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 669–674.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196756 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Lavie, N., Hirst, A., de Fockert, J. W., & Viding, E. (2004). Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 339–354.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.133.3.339 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. McHugo, M., Olatunji, B. O., & Zald, D. H. (2013). The emotional attentional blink: What we know so far. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 151.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00151 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Most, S. B., Chun, M. M., Widders, D. M., & Zald, D. H. (2005). Attentional rubbernecking: Cognitive control and personality in emotion-induced blindness. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 654–661.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196754 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Most, S. B., & Jungé, J. A. (2008). Don’t look back: Retroactive, dynamic costs and benefits of emotional capture. Visual Cognition, 16, 262–278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Most, S. B., Smith, S. D., Cooter, A. B., Levy, B. N., & Zald, D. H. (2007). The naked truth: Positive, arousing distractors impair rapid target perception. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 964–981.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Most, S. B., & Wang, L. (2011). Dissociating spatial attention and awareness in emotion-induced blindness. Psychological Science, 22, 300–305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Müller, M. M., Andersen, S. K., & Keil, A. (2008). Time course of competition for visual processing resources between emotional pictures and foreground task. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 1892–1899.  https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhm215 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Nieuwenhuis, S., & de Kleijn, R. (2011). Consciousness of targets during the attentional blink: A gradual or all-or-none dimension? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 364–373.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-010-0026-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Olatunji, B. O., Ciesielski, B. G., Armstrong, T., Zhao, M., & Zald, D. H. (2011). Making something out of nothing: Neutral content modulates attention in generalized anxiety disorder. Depression and Anxiety, 28, 427–434.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Olivers, C. N. L., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2006). The beneficial effects of additional task load, positive affect, and instruction on the attentional blink. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 364–379.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.32.2.364 Google Scholar
  31. Overgaard, M., Rote, J., Mouridsen, K., & Ramsøy, T. Z. (2006). Is conscious perception gradual or dichotomous? A comparison of report methodologies during a visual task. Consciousness and Cognition, 15, 700–708.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Park, S., Kim, M.-S., & Chun, M. M. (2007). Concurrent working memory load can facilitate selective attention: Evidence for specialized load. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 1062–1075.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.20.5.1062 Google Scholar
  33. Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10, 437–442.  https://doi.org/10.1163/156856897X00366 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Phelps, E. A., Ling, S., & Carrasco, M. (2006). Emotion facilitates perception and potentiates the perceptual benefits of attention. Psychological Science, 17, 292–299.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01701.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Piech, R., McHugo, M., Smith, S., Dukic, M., Meer, J., Abou-Khalil, B., ... Zald, D. (2011). Attentional capture by emotional stimuli is preserved in patients with amygdala lesions. Neuropsychologia, 49, 3314–3319.Google Scholar
  36. Piech, R., Pastorino, M., & Zald, D. (2010). All I saw was the cake: Hunger effects on attentional capture by visual food cues. Appetite, 54, 579–582.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Pourtois, G., Grandjean, D., Sander, D., & Vuilleumier, P. (2004). Electrophysiological correlates of rapid spatial orienting toward fearful faces. Cerebral Cortex, 14, 619–633.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Ramsøy, T. Z., & Overgaard, M. (2004). Introspection and subliminal perception. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 3, 1–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Raymond, J. E., Shapiro, K. L., & Arnell, K. M. (1992). Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: An attentional blink? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 849–860.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.18.3.849 Google Scholar
  40. Schönwald, L. I., & Müller, M. M. (2014). Slow biasing of processing resources in early visual cortex is preceded by emotional cue extraction in emotion–attention competition. Human Brain Mapping, 35, 1477–1490.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Sergent, C., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Is consciousness a gradual phenomenon? Evidence for an all-or-none bifurcation during the attentional blink. Psychological Science, 15, 720–728.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Smith, S. D., Most, S. B., Newsome, L. A., & Zald, D. H. (2006). An emotion-induced attentional blink elicited by aversively conditioned stimuli. Emotion, 6, 523–527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Sy, J. L., Marois, R., & Tong, F. (2018). Conscious perception can be both graded and discrete. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  44. Theeuwes, J., Atchley, P., & Kramer, A. F. (2000). On the time course of top-down and bottom-up control of visual attention. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII (pp. 105–124). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  45. Theeuwes, J., & Van der Stigchel, S. (2006). Faces capture attention: Evidence from inhibition of return. Visual Cognition, 13, 657–665.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2001). Effects of attention and emotion on face processing in the human brain: An event-related fMRI study. Neuron, 30, 829–841.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00328-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Vuilleumier, P., & Schwartz, S. (2001). Emotional facial expressions capture attention. Neurology, 56, 153–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Wang, L., Kennedy, B. L., & Most, S. B. (2012). When emotion blinds: A spatiotemporal competition account of emotion-induced blindness. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 438.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00438 Google Scholar
  49. Wierda, S. M., van Rijn, H., Taatgen, N. A., & Martens, S. (2010). Distracting the mind improves performance: An ERP study. PLoS ONE, 5, 15024.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0015024 CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© The Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of California, San DiegoLa JollaUSA
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyVanderbilt UniversityNashvilleUSA

Personalised recommendations