Advertisement

Animal learning & behavior

, Volume 10, Issue 3, pp 314–320 | Cite as

Food aversion learning in kangaroo rats: A specialist-generalist comparison

  • Martin Daly
  • Joan Rauschenberger
  • Phil Behrends
Article

Abstract

Two species of kangaroo rat (Dipodomys), varying in their degree of dietary specialization, were compared in a series of food aversion learning experiments to test the hypothesis that rapid aversion learning is an adaptation of relatively generalist feeders. The more generalist species indeed learned better or more rapidly in certain experiments, but species differences were at least partly a function of the specific test foods. Interpretation of results is complicated by differences between the two species in their initial reactions to particular foods, in the relative efficacy of different foods in supporting learned aversions, and perhaps even in their physiological responses to illness-inducing and control procedures.

Reference Note

  1. 1.
    Harper, K. J., Kenagy, G. J., & Oakley,G. The gustatory responses of two species of kangaroo rats. Paper presented to the Michigan chapter of the Society for Neuroscience, 1976.Google Scholar

References

  1. Bradley, W. G., &Mauer, B. A. Reproduction and food habits of Merriam’s kangaroo rat,Dipodomys merriami.Journal of Mammalogy, 1971,52, 497–507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Brown, J. H., Reichman, O. J., &Davidson, D. W. Granivory in desert ecosystems.Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 1979,10, 207–227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Chitty, D. The control of rats and mice. Oxford: Clarendon, 1954.Google Scholar
  4. Csuti, B. A. Patterns of adaptation and variation in the Great Basin kangaroo rat (Dipodomys microps).University of California Publications in Zoology, 1979,111, viii + 69.Google Scholar
  5. Garcia, J., Ervin, R. R., &Koelling, R. A. Learning with prolonged delay of reinforcement.Psychonomic Science, 1966,5, 121–122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Garcia, J., &Koelling, R. A. Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning.Psychonomic Science, 1966,4, 123–124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Hinde, R. A., &Stevenson-Hinde, J. (Eds.)Constraints on learning: Limitations and predispositions. London: Academic Press, 1973.Google Scholar
  8. Johnson, D. R. The food habits of rodents on rangelands of southern Idaho.Ecology, 1961,42, 407–410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Kenagy, G. J. Saltbush leaves: Excision of hypersaline tissue by a kangaroo rat.Science, 1972,178, 1094–1096.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Kenagy, G. J. Adaptations for leaf-eating in the Great Basin kangaroo ratDipodomys microps.Oecologia, 1973,12, 383–412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Mullen, R. K. Energy metabolism and body water turnover rates of two species of free-living kangaroo rats,Dipodomys merriami andDipodomys microps.Comparative Biochemistry & Physiology, 1971,39A, 379–390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Rozin, P. The selection of foods by rats, humans and other animals. In J. S. Rosenblatt, R. A. Hinde, E. Shaw, & C. Beer (Eds.),Advances in the study of behavior (Vol. 6). London: Academic Press, 1976.Google Scholar
  13. Seligman, M. E. P. On the generality of the laws of learning.Psychological Review, 1970,77, 406–418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Seligman, M. E. P., &Hager, J. (Eds.)The biological boundaries of learning. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1973.Google Scholar
  15. Sherbrooke, W. C. Differential acceptance of toxic jojoba seed (Simmondsia chinensis) by four Sonoran desert heteromyid rodents.Ecology, 1976,57, 596–602.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 1982

Authors and Affiliations

  • Martin Daly
    • 1
    • 2
  • Joan Rauschenberger
    • 1
    • 2
  • Phil Behrends
    • 1
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyMcMaster UniversityHamiltonCanada
  2. 2.University of CaliforniaRiverside

Personalised recommendations