Advertisement

Animal learning & behavior

, Volume 10, Issue 3, pp 301–304 | Cite as

Discrimination reversal learning in newts

  • Stuart R. Ellins
  • Robert E. Cramer
  • Glenn C. Martin
Article
  • 205 Downloads

Abstract

The results of field research suggest that, contrary to being behaviorally inflexible, some amphibians may have the ability to respond effectively to changing environments. The performance of seven newts (Triturus viridescens) was studied across 20 successive reversals of a spatial discrimination problem in a dry T-maze. Submersion in shaded water served as reinforcement for correct responses. The subjects showed a decrease in mean errors across reversals and across ordinal trials within sessions. These results are discussed in terms of the importance of using biologically relevant methodologies in the study of comparative animal learning.

References

  1. Bitterman, M. E. The evolution of intelligence.Scientific American, 1965,212, 92–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bitterman, M. E. Animal learning. In J. B. Sidowski (Ed.),Experimental methods and instrumentation in psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1966.Google Scholar
  3. Bitterman, M. E. The comparative analysis of learning. Are the laws of learning the same in all animals?Science, 1975,20, 699–709.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Deyrup, I. J. Water balance and kidney. In J. A. Moore (Ed.),Physiology of the amphibia. New York: Academic Press, 1964.Google Scholar
  5. Ellins, S. R., &Masterson, F. A. Successive discrimination reversals in the bat.Psychonomic Science, 1971,25, 265–266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Gossette, R. Comparisons of successive discrimination reversal (SDR) performances across fifteen different avian and mammalian species.American Zoologist, 1966,6, 157.Google Scholar
  7. Gossette, R. Variation in magnitude of negative transfer on successive discrimination reversal (SDR) tasks across species.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1969,29, 803–811.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Gossette, R., &Cohen, H. Error reduction by pigeons on a spatial successive reversal task under conditions of non-correction.Psychological Reports, 1966,18, 367–370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Gossette, R., Gossette, M., &Inman, N. Successive discrimination reversal performance by the greater hill myna.Animal Behavior, 1966,14, 50–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Gossette, R., &Hombach, A. Successive discrimination reversal (SDR) performances of American alligators and American crocodiles on a spatial task.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1969,28, 63–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Gossette, R., &Inman, N. Comparison of spatial successive discrimination reversal performance of two groups of New World monkeys.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1966,23, 169–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Grant, D., Anderson, O., &Twitty, V. Homing orientation by olfaction in newts (Taricha rivularis).Science, 1968,160, 1354–1355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Hershkowitz, M., &Samuel, D. The retention of learning during metamorphosis of the crested newt (Triturus cristatus).Animal Behavior, 1973,21, 83–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Hodos, W., &Campbell, C. B. G. Scala naturae: Why there is no theory in comparative psychology.Psychological Review, 1969,76, 337–350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Karplus, I., Algom, D., &Samuel, D. Acquisition and retention of dark avoidance by the toad,Xenopus laevis (Daudin).Animal Learning & Behavior, 1981,9, 45–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Landreth, H. F., &Ferguson, D. E. Newts: Sun compass orientation.Science, 1967,158, 1459–1461.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Ray, A. J. Avoidance learning in the tiger salamander.American Journal of Psychology, 1967,80, 642–643.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Ray, A. J. Instrumental avoidance learning by the tiger salamander,Ambystoma tigrinum.Animal Behavior, 1970,18, 73–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Segal, M., Hershkowitz, M., Samuel, D., &Bitterman, M. E. Rapid modification of amphibian behavior by punishment.Psychonomic Science, 1971,24, 249–250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Seidman, E. Relative ability of the newt and terrapin to reverse a direction habit.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1949,42, 320–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Seligman, M. E. P. On the generality of the laws of learning.Psychological Review, 1970,77, 406–418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Thompson, P. A., &Boice, R. Attempts to train frogs: Review and experiments.Journal of Biological Psychology, 1975,17, 3–13.Google Scholar
  23. Thorpe, W. H. Learning and instinct in animals. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1963.Google Scholar
  24. Twitty, V. C. Migration and speciation in newts.Science, 1959,130, 1735–1743.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Twitty, V. C., Grant, D., &Anderson, O. Course and timing of the homing migration in the newtTaricha rivularis.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 1964,56, 864.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Twitty, V. C., Grant, D., &Anderson, O. Initial homeward orientation after long-distance displacements in the newtTaricha rivularis.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 1967,57, 342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 1982

Authors and Affiliations

  • Stuart R. Ellins
    • 1
  • Robert E. Cramer
    • 1
  • Glenn C. Martin
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyCalifornia State CollegeSan Bernardino, San Bernardino

Personalised recommendations