Skip to main content
Log in

Criminalizing the brain: Neurocriminology and the production of strategic ignorance

  • Original Article
  • Published:
BioSocieties Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

With the increasing use of imaging technologies like fMRI in prison sentencing and penal policy, sociologists must comprehend the consequences of these trends and the scientific assumptions upon which they stand. This article uses insights from the sociology of knowledge to interrogate the epistemological and ontological assumptions of neurocriminology, an interdisciplinary field that studies the neural basis of crime. Through a discourse analysis of research articles that embrace what we term the “neurocriminological vision,” we demonstrate how features of the research design eschew the consideration of social factors underlying crime and antisocial behavior. Focusing on the selection of control variables, the ‘thinness’ of experimental tasks, and the management of inconvenient facts, we demonstrate how neurocriminological research transforms complex, socially situated behaviors into problems of neurocircuitry. We link these practices to the field-specific dynamics in which neurocriminology is situated, specifically as an interdisciplinary field which derives authority from neuroscience but is met with skepticism within criminology. In response to these dynamics, neurocriminologists produce not only knowledge, but also ignorance that is strategically useful given their professional goals. Beyond the particular case at hand, we emphasize the relationship between internal dynamics within scientific fields and their effects on the co-production of knowledge and ignorance.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We carried out an analysis of the disciplinary affiliations of the first authors for each article in our sample. We used their PhD discipline as indicative of their primary affiliation. The majority of articles in our sample were written by researchers with PhDs in psychology (n = 40). The remaining authors are affiliated with psychiatry (n = 16), neuroscience (7), medical fields (neurology, neurosurgery, and neuroradiology) (7), criminology (2), law (2), health science (1), history (1), and philosophy (1).

  2. Neurocriminologists interface with behavioral genetics, neuroscience proper, psychology, and other neurodisciplines like “neurolaw” (Jones et al. 2013), “neuroethics” (Roskies 2002), and neuroscientific research on morality (Heekeren et al. 2003), but the primary orientation of its knowledge claims are toward criminological research.

  3. We conducted a search of these journals for the keyword “fMRI” and found only 16 mentions since 1990. Fifteen of these mentions occurred in Criminal Justice and Behavior, which also ran a special issue on Biosocial Criminology in November 2009. This signals that the applied criminal justice wing of criminology is slightly more amenable to neurocriminology than its social science counterparts.

  4. For example, Kiehl works in close concert with the New Mexico State Correctional Department (Kiehl 2014). Previously, his research was supported by the Correctional Services of Canada.

  5. We conducted the following keyword searches: “antisocial personality AND fMRI”; “psychopathy AND fMRI”; “antisocial personality AND brain imaging”; “psychopathy AND brain imaging”; “conduct disorder AND fMRI”; “conduct disorder AND brain imaging.” We also did an initial search of the keyword “neurocriminology” but this search yielded mostly review articles, rather than the research articles that are the focus of our analysis. Similarly, searches such as “criminal behavior AND fMRI” did not yield as many substantive results as the other keyword searches.

  6. We excluded articles that used PET and SPECT methods in their research design. Nevertheless, insofar as event-related imaging research shares similar logic, our analysis can be extended to research using technologies other than fMRI such as Positron Emission Topography (PET) and Single-Photon Emission Computed Tomography (SPECT).

References

  • Abend, G. 2011. Thick concepts and the moral brain. European Journal of Sociology. 52 (1): 143–172.

    Google Scholar 

  • Abend, G. 2016. What are neural correlates neural correlates of? BioSocieties 12 (3): 1–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Adolphs, R. 2003. Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 4 (3): 165–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aharoni, E., G.M. Vincent, C.L. Harenski, V.D. Calhoun, W. Sinnott-Armstrong, M.S. Gazzinga, and K.A. Kiehl. 2013. Neuroprediction of future rearrest. Proceedings of the National academy of Sciences of the United States of America 110 (15): 6223–6228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Albein-Urios, N., J. Verdejo-Román, C. Soriano-Mas, S. Asensio, J.M. Martínez-González, and A. Verdejo-García. 2013. Cocaine users with comorbid Cluster B personality disorders show dysfunctional brain activation and connectivity in the emotional regulation networks during negative emotion maintenance and reappraisal. European Neuropsychopharmacology 23 (12): 1698–1707.

    Google Scholar 

  • Albert, M., and D.L. Kleinman. 2011. Bringing Pierre Bourdieu to science and technology studies. Minerva 49 (3): 263–273.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, M. 2010. The new Jim Crow: mass incarceration in the age of colorblindness. New York: The New Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barkataki, I.K., V. Kumari, M. Das, A. Sumich, P. Taylor, and T. Sharma. 2008. Neural correlates of deficient response inhibition in mentally disordered violent individuals. Behavioral Sciences & the Law 26: 51–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beaver, K.M., J.L. Nedelec, C. da Silva Costa, and M.M. Vidal. 2015. The future of biosocial criminology. Criminal Justice Studies 28 (1): 6–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bediou, B., M. Eimer, T. d’Amato, T. d’Amato, O. Hauk, and A.J. Calder. 2009. In the eye of the beholder: Individual differences in reward-drive modulate early frontocentral ERPs to angry faces. Neuropsychologia 47 (3): 825–834.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bourdieu, P. 1975. The specificity of the scientific field and the social conditions of the progress of reason. Social Science Information 14 (6): 19–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brekhus, W. 1998. A sociology of the unmarked: Redirecting our focus. Sociological Theory 16 (1): 34–51.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, C., and M. Michael. 2014. Enacting the ‘neuro’ in practice: Translational research, adhesion and the promise of porosity. Social Studies of Science 44 (5): 680–700.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burge, T. 1986. Individualism and psychology. Philosophical Review 45: 3–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burri, R.V. 2008. Doing distinctions: Boundary work and symbolic capital in radiology. Social Studies of Science 38 (1): 35–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burt, C.H., and R.L. Simons. 2014. Pulling back the curtain on heritability studies: Biosocial criminology in the postgenomic era. Criminology 52 (2): 223–262.

    Google Scholar 

  • Button, K., J. Ioannidis, C. Mokrysz, B. Nosek, J. Flint, E. Robinson, and M. Munafò. 2013. Power failure: Why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 14: 365–376.

    Google Scholar 

  • Contreras-Rodríguez, O., J. Pujol, I. Batalla, B.J. Harrison, J. Bosque, I. Ibern-Regàs, R. Hernández-Ribas, C. Soriano-Mas, J. Deus, and M. López-Solà. 2014. Disrupted neural processing of emotional faces in psychopathy. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 9 (4): 505–512.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, G. 2013. Secrets of the criminal mind. Scientific American, 7 May, https://www.scientificamerican.com/article/secrets-criminal-mind-adrian-raine/. Accessed 16 March 2018.

  • Danziger, K. 1990. Constructing the subject: Historical origins of psychological research. NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daston, L., and P. Galison. 2010. Objectivity. Boston: Zone Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dumit, J. 2004. Picturing personhood: Brain scans and biomedical identity. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Easterbrook, P.J., R. Gopalan, J.A. Berlin, and D.R. Matthews. 1991. Publication bias in clinical research. The Lancet 337 (8746): 867–872.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, T., A. Achtziger, G. Roth, and D. Strüber. 2014. Neural correlates of empathetic perceptual processing of realistic social interaction scenarios displayed from a first-order perspective. Brain Research 1583: 141–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fine, A., L. Steinberg, P.J. Frick, and E. Cauffman. 2016. Self-control assessments and implications for predicting adolescent offending. Journal of Youth and Adolescence 45: 701–712.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, G.N., and M. Mulkay. 1984. Opening Pandora’s box: A sociological analysis of scientists’ discourse. NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glenn, A.L., and A. Raine. 2014. Neurocriminology: Implications for the punishment, prediction and prevention of criminal behavior. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 15: 54–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. 1994. Professional vision. American Anthropologist 96 (3): 606–633.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hacking, I. 1992. The self-vindication of the laboratory sciences. In Science as practice and culture, ed. A. Pickering, 29–64. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hare, R.D. 2007. The Hare psychopathy checklist-revised: PLC-R. Toronto: MHS, Multi-Health Systems.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harenski, C.L., and K.A. Kiehl. 2010. Neural processing of moral violations among incarcerated adolescents with psychopathic traits. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 10: 181–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartung, C.M., and T.A. Widiger. 1998. Gender differences in the diagnosis of mental disorders: Conclusions and controversies of the DSMIV. Psychological Bulletin 123 (3): 260–278.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hedgecoe, A. 2001. Schizophrenia and the narrative of enlightened geneticization. Social Studies of Science 31 (6): 875–911.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heekeren, H.R., I. Wartenburger, H. Schmidt, H. Schwintowski, and A. Villringer. 2003. An fMRI study of simple ethical decision-making. NeuroReport 14 (9): 1215–1219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heimer, C.A. 2012. Inert facts and the illusion of knowledge: Strategic uses of ignorance in HIV clinics. Economy and Society 41 (1): 17–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herpetz, S.C., and H. Sass. 2000. Emotional deficiency and psychopathy. Behavioral Sciences & the Law 18: 567–580.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heylen, B., L. Pauwels, K.M. Beaver, and M. Ruffinengo. 2015. Defending biosocial criminology: On the discursive style of our critics, the separation of ideology and science, and a biologically informed defense of fundamental values. Journal of Theoretical & Philosophical Criminology 7 (1): 83–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, V. 2010. Science in court: Head case. Nature News 464 (7287): 340–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, O.D., R. Marois, M.J. Farah, and H.T. Greely. 2013. Law and neuroscience. Journal of Neuroscience 33 (45): 17624–17630.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joyce, K.A. 2008. Magnetic appeal: MRI and the myth of transparency. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiehl, K.A. 2014. The psychopath whisperer: The science of those without conscience. New York: Crown Publishing Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiehl, K.A., and M.B. Hoffman. 2011. The criminal psychopath: History, neuroscience, treatment, and economics. Jurimetrics 51: 355–397.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiehl, K.A., A.M. Smith, A. Mendrek, B.B. Forster, R.D. Hare, and P.F. Liddle. 2004. Temporal lobe abnormalities in semantic processing by criminal psychopaths as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 130 (1): 27–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. 1988. The pasteurization of France. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levitt, S.D., and J.A. List. 2007. What do laboratory experiments measuring social preferences reveal about the real world? The Journal of Economic Perspectives 21: 153–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, B.T., and B.J. Ham. 2008. Monoamine oxidase A-uVNTR genotype affects limbic brain activity in response to affective facial stimuli. Behavioral, Integrative and Clinical Neuroscience 19 (5): 515–519.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, M.D. 2013. Social: Why our brains are wired to connect. NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Littlefield, M.M., and J. Johnson. 2012. The neuroscientific turn: Transdisciplinarity in the age of the brain. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Macaskill, G. 2015. Brain scan can identify serial killers-in-waiting: Experts reveal everyday triggers that turn people to crime. Mirror, 20 October, https://www.mirror.co.uk/news/uk-news/brain-scan-can-identify-serial-6641868. Accessed 16 March 2018.

  • Marris, C., C. Jefferson, and F. Lentzos. 2014. Negotiating the dynamics of uncomfortable knowledge: The case of dual use and synthetic biology. BioSocieties 9 (4): 393–420.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGoey, L. 2012. Strategic unknowns: Towards a sociology of ignorance. Economy and Society 41 (1): 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. 2016. Brain scans are prone to false positives, study says. Science 353 (6296): 208–209.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moskowitz, C. 2011. Criminal minds are different from yours, brain scans reveal. LiveScience, 4 March, https://www.livescience.com/13083-criminals-brain-neuroscience-ethics.html. Accessed 16 March 2018.

  • Mullaney, J.L. 1999. Making it “count”: Mental weighing and identity attribution. Symbolic Interaction 22 (3): 269–283.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nock, M.K., A.E. Kazdin, E. Hiripi, and R.C. Kessler. 2006. Prevalence, subtypes, and correlates of DSM-IV conduct disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Psychological Medicine 36 (5): 699–710.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nordstrom, B., Y. Gao, A. Glenn, M. Peskin, A. Rudo-Hutt, R. Schug, Y. Yang, and A. Raine. 2011. Neurocriminology. Advances in Genetics 75: 255–283.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norgaard, K.M. 2006. ‘People want to protect themselves a little bit’: Emotions, denial, and social movement nonparticipation. Sociological Inquiry 76 (3): 372–396.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Connor, C., G. Rees, and H. Joffe. 2012. Neuroscience in the public sphere. Neuron 74 (2): 220–226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ortega, F., and F. Vidal. 2011. Neurocultures: Glimpses into an expanding universe. NY: Peter Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osumi, T., T. Nakao, Y. Kasuya, J. Shinoda, J. Yamada, and H. Ohira. 2012. Amygdala dysfunction attenuates frustration-induced aggression in psychopathic individuals in a non-criminal population. Journal of Affective Disorders 142 (1): 331–338.

    Google Scholar 

  • Panofsky, A. 2011. Field analysis and interdisciplinary science: Scientific capital exchange in behavior genetics. Minerva 49 (3): 295–316.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, A., A. Amedi, F. Fregni, and L.B. Merabet. 2005. The plastic human brain cortex. Annual Reviews of Neuroscience. 28: 377–401.

    Google Scholar 

  • Payer, D.E., K. Baicy, M.D. Lieberman, and E.D. London. 2012. Overlapping neural substrates between intentional and incidental down-regulation of negative emotions. Emotion 12 (2): 229–235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, A. 2001. Biofantasies: Genetics and medicine in the print media. Social Science and Medicine 52 (8): 1255–1268.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pickersgill, M. 2011a. ‘Promising’ therapies: Neuroscience, clinical practice, and the treatment of psychopathy. Sociology of Health & Illness 33 (3): 448–464.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pickersgill, M. 2011b. Ordering disorder: Knowledge production and uncertainty in neuroscience research. Science as Culture 20 (1): 71–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pitts-Taylor, V. 2016. The brain’s body: Neuroscience and corporeal politics. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plomin, R.D., J.C. DeFries, G.E. McClearn, and P. McGuffin (eds.). 2001. Behavioral genetics: A primer, 4th ed. New York: Worth Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raine, A. 2013. The anatomy of violence: The biological roots of crime. NY: Pantheon Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Racine, E., S. Waldman, J. Rosenberg, and J. Iles. 2010. Contemporary neuroscience in the media. Social Science and Medicine 71 (4): 725–733.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rafter, N. 2008. The criminal brain. New York, NY: New York University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reisig, M.D., W.D. Bales, C. Hay, and X. Wang. 2007. The effect of racial inequality on black male recidivism. Justice Quarterly 24 (3): 408–434.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rocque, M., and C. Posick. 2017. Paradigm shift or normal science? The future of (biosocial) criminology. Theoretical Criminology 21 (3): 288–303.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rose, N., and J.M. Abi-Rached. 2013. Neuro: The new brain sciences and the management of the mind. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roskies, A. 2002. Neuroethics for the new millennium. Neuron 35 (1): 21–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubia, K., R. Halari, A.B. Smith, F. Matsukura, M. Mohammad, E. Taylor, and M. Brammer. 2008. Dissociated functional brain abnormalities of inhibition in boys with pure conduct disorder and in boys with pure attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 165 (7): 889–897.

    Google Scholar 

  • Savelsberg, J.J., R.D. King, and L.L. Cleveland. 2002. Politicized scholarship? Science on crime and the state. Social Problems 49 (3): 327–348.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sbordone, R.J. 1996. Ecological validity: Some critical issues for the neuropsychologist. In Ecological validity of neuropsychological testing, ed. R.J. Sborbone and C.J. Long, 15–41. Delray Beach: St Lucie Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scargle, J.D. 2000. Publication bias: The “file-drawer” problem in scientific inference. Journal of Scientific Exploration 14 (1): 91–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, S., J. Peters, U. Bromberg, S. Brassen, M.M. Menz, S.F. Miedl, E. Loth, T. Banaschewski, A. Barbot, G. Barker, P.J. Conrod, J.W. Dalley, H. Flor, J. Gallinat, H. Garavan, A. Heinz, B. Itterman, C. Mallik, K. Mann, E. Artiges, T. Paus, J.B. Poline, M. Rietschel, L. Reed, M.N. Smolka, R. Spanagel, C. Speiser, A. Ströhle, M. Struve, G. Schumann, C. Büchel, and the IMAGEN consortium. 2011. Boys do it the right way: Sex-dependent amygdala lateralization during face processing in adolescents. NeuroImage 56: 1847–1853.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seabrook, J. 2008. Suffering souls. The New Yorker, 10 November, http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2008/11/10/suffering-souls. accessed 25 August 2017.

  • Sherman, L.W., D.A. Smith, J.D. Schmidt, and D.P. Rogan. 1992. Crime, punishment, and stake in conformity: Legal and informal control of domestic violence. American Sociological Review 57 (5): 680.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, A.B., E. Taylor, M. Brammer, R. Halari, and K. Rubia. 2008. Reduced activation in right lateral prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate gyrus in medication-naïve adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder during time discrimination. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 49 (9): 977–985.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sommer, M., B. Sodian, K. Döhnel, J. Schwerdtner, J. Meinhardt, and G. Hajak. 2010. In psychopathic patients emotion attribution modulates activity in outcome-related brain areas. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 182 (2): 88–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thornton, D.J. 2011. Brain culture: Neuroscience and popular media. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Uttal, W. 2001. The new phrenology: The limits of localizing cognitive processes in the brain. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vidal, F. 2009. Brainhood, anthropological figure of modernity. History of the Human Sciences 22 (1): 5–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Völlm, B., P. Richardson, S. McKie, R. Elliott, M. Dolan, and B. Deakin. 2007. Neuronal correlates of reward and loss in cluster B personality disorders: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 156 (2): 151–167.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, X., D. Mears, and W. Bales. 2010. Race-specific employment contexts and recidivism. Criminology 48 (4): 1171–1211.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weber, M. 1958. Science as a vocation. Daedalus 87 (1): 111–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiesberg, D.S., F. Keil, J. Goodstein, E. Rawson, and J.R. Gray. 2008. The seductive allure of neuroscience explanations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 20 (3): 470–477.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woolgar, S., and D. Pawluch. 1985. Ontological gerrymandering: The anatomy of social problems explanations. Social Problems 32 (3): 214–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, J.P., and D. Boisvert. 2009. What biosocial criminology offers criminology. Criminal Justice and Behavior 36 (11): 1228–1240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wynn, R., M.H. Holseth, and G. Petterson. 2012. Psychopathy in women: Theoretical and clinical perspectives. International Journal of Women’s Health 4: 257–263.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zerubavel, E. 2015. Hidden in plain sight: The social structure of irrelevance. NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mallory Fallin.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Fallin, M., Whooley, O. & Barker, K.K. Criminalizing the brain: Neurocriminology and the production of strategic ignorance. BioSocieties 14, 438–462 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1057/s41292-018-0135-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1057/s41292-018-0135-y

Keywords

Navigation