Skip to main content
Log in

The Relationship Between Delay and Social Discounting, and Body Mass Index in University Students

  • Original Article
  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Delay discounting is associated with body mass index (BMI), with individuals who discount more having higher BMIs. This same relationship was found when a social component was added during a social temporal discounting procedure. However, no study has tested whether the social component by itself is related to BMI. The current study tested the relationships between social discounting, delay discounting, and BMI. Eight hundred seven undergraduates completed delay and social discounting measures, and a demographic questionnaire from which BMI was calculated. Social discounting was not significantly related to age-adjusted or non-age-adjusted BMI. However, using non-age-adjusted BMI scores and dichotomizing individuals as either non-obese/obese resulted in a significant relationship between social discounting and BMI in the predicted direction, whereby participants with a higher BMI shared significantly more than non-obese individuals. Consistent with previous research, there was a significant relationship between delay discounting and BMI, and a significant correlation between delay and social discounting. Additionally, participants with low BMI (< 18.5) appeared to substantially contribute to the significant results. The current results replicated a previous delay discounting and BMI relationship, but also demonstrate the need to standardize how BMI is calculated and focus on participants with low BMI in relation to discounting measures.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Analyses were re-run including all participants that provided a delay and social discounting score (i.e., including all inconsistent and non-systematic discounters), but without the 14 participants with invalid social discounting SurveyMonkey data (n = 787). This analyses showed one different outcome. Unlike the results reported with the exclusion criteria in place (n = 437), there was not a statistically significant difference for the non-adjusted BMI delay discounting outcome F(3, 783) = 1.64, p = 0.179. However, when collapsing BMI groups into either lower or higher weight, this difference was now statistically significant t(785) = − 2.25, p = 0.025. Consistent with the results using the exclusion criteria, there was not a significant difference for the non-adjusted BMI social discounting outcome F(3, 783) = 2.166, p = 0.091. Collapsing the BMI groups into either lower or higher weight still resulted in a significant social discounting difference t(785) = − 1.449, p = 0.015.

References

  • Andrade, L. F., & Petry, N. M. (2014). White problem gamblers discount delayed rewards less steeply than their African American and Hispanic counterparts. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 28(2), 599–606.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bickel, W. K., Jarmolowicz, D. P., Mueller, E. T., Koffarnus, M. N., & Gatchalian, K. M. (2012). Excessive discounting of delayed reinforcers as a trans-disease process contributing to addiction and other disease-related vulnerabilities: emerging evidence. Pharmacology & Therapeutics, 134, 287–297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bickel, W. K., Wilson, A. G., Franck, C. T., Mueller, E. T., Jarmolowicz, D. P., Koffarnus, M. N., et al. (2014). Using crowdsourcing to compare temporal, social temporal, and probability discounting among obese and non-obese individuals. Appetite, 75, 82–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bickel, W. K., Yi, R., Landes, R. D., Hill, P. F., & Baxter, C. (2011). Remember the future: working memory training decreases delay discounting among stimulant addicts. Biological Psychiatry, 69(3), 260–265. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.08.017.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Borges, A. M., Kuang, J., Milhorn, H., & Yi, R. (2016). An alternative approach to calculating area-under-the-curve (AUC) in delay discounting research. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 106, 145–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruce, A. S., Black, W. R., Bruce, J. M., Daldalian, M., Martin, L. E., & Davis, A. M. (2011). Ability to delay gratification and BMI in preadolescence. Obesity, 19(5), 1101–1102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2015a). About adult BMI. Retrieved from https://www.cdc.gov/healthyweight/assessing/bmi/adult_bmi/index.html

  • Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2015b). About child and teen BMI. Retrieved from https://www.cdc.gov/healthyweight/assessing/bmi/childrens_bmi/about_childrens_ bmi.html.

  • Dahl, A. K., Reynolds, C. A., Fall, T., Magnusson, P. K. E., & Pedersen, N. L. (2014). Multifactorial analysis of changes in body mass index across the adult life course: a study with 65 years of follow-up. International Journal of Obesity, 38(8), 1133–1141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Castro, J. M. (1994). Family and friends produce greater social facilitation of food intake than other companions. Physiology & Behavior, 56(3), 445–455.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Castro, J. M., King, G. A., Duarte-Gardea, M., Gonzalez-Ayala, S., & Kooshian, C. H. (2012). Overweight and obese humans overeat away from home. Appetite, 59(2), 204–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Water, E., Cillessen, A. H. N., & Scheres, A. (2014). Distinct age-related differences in temporal discounting and risk taking in adolescents and young adults. Child Development, 85(5), 1881–1897.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Wit, H., Flory, J. D., Acheson, A., McCloskey, M., & Manuck, S. B. (2007). IQ and nonplanning impulsivity are independently associated with delay discounting in middle-aged adults. Personality and Individual Differences, 42(1), 111–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dickman, S. J. (1990). Functional and dysfunctional impulsivity: personality and cognitive correlates. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(1), 95–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Du, W., Green, L., & Myerson, J. (2002). Cross-cultural comparisons of discounting delayed and probabilistic rewards. The Psychological Record, 52(4), 479–492.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, L. H., Jankowiak, N., Fletcher, K. D., Carr, K. A., Nederkoorn, C., Raynor, H. A., & Finkelstein, E. (2014). Women who are motivated to eat and discount the future are more obese. Obesity, 22(6), 1394–1399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fairburn, C. G., Cooper, Z., Shafran, R., Bohn, K., Hawker, D. M., Murphy, R., & Straebler, S. (2008). Underweight and undereating. Cognitive behavior therapy and eating disorders; cognitive behavior therapy and eating disorders (pp. 147–182, Chapter xii, 324 Pages) Guilford Press, New York.

  • Fields, S. A., Sabet, M., Peal, A., & Reynolds, B. (2011). Relationship between weight status and delay discounting in a sample of adolescent cigarette smokers. Behavioural Pharmacology, 22(3), 266–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hendrickson, K. L., & Rasmussen, E. B. (2017). Mindful eating reduces impulsive food choice in adolescents and adults. Health Psychology, 36(3), 226–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howland, M., Hunger, J. M., & Mann, T. (2012). Friends don’t let friends eat cookies: effects of restrictive eating norms on consumption among friends. Appetite, 59(2), 505–509.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, A. S., Janssen, I., Sui, X., Church, T. S., & Blair, S. N. (2012). Longitudinal changes in body composition associated with healthy ageing: men, aged 20-96 years. The British Journal of Nutrition, 107(7), 1085–1091.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. W., & Bickel, W. K. (2002). Within-subject comparison of real and hypothetical money rewards in delay discounting. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 77(2), 129–146. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2002.77-129.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. W., & Bickel, W. K. (2008). An algorithm for identifying nonsystematic delay-discounting data. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 16, 264–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, B. A., & Rachlin, H. (2006). Social discounting. Psychological Science, 17, 283–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, B. A., & Rachlin, H. (2009). Delay, probability, and social discounting in a public goods game. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 91, 61–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaisari, P., & Higgs, S. (2015). Social modelling of food intake. The role of familiarity of the dining partners and food type. Appetite, 86, 19–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, B. A., Amlung, M., Reed, D. D., Jarmolowicz, D. P., McKerchar, T. L., & Lemley, S. M. (2016). Automating scoring of delay discounting for the 21- and 27-item monetary choice questionnaires. The Behavior Analyst, 39(2), 293–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, K. N., Petry, N. M., & Bickel, W. K. (1999). Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128(1), 78–87. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.128.1.78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koffarnus, M. N., Jarmolowicz, D. P., Mueller, E. T., & Bickel, W. K. (2013). Changing delay discounting in the light of the competing neurobehavioral decision systems theory: a review. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 99(1), 32–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koh, J., & Pliner, P. (2009). The effects of degree of acquaintance, plate size, and sharing on food intake. Appetite, 52(3), 595–602.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kulendran, M., Vlaev, I., Sugden, C., King, D., Ashrafian, H., Gately, P., & Darzi, A. (2014). Neuropsychological assessment as a predictor of weight loss in obese adolescents. International Journal of Obesity, 38(4), 507–512. https://doi.org/10.1038/ijo.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lemley, S. M., Kaplan, B. A., Reed, D. D., Darden, A. C., & Jarmolowicz, D. P. (2016). Reinforcer pathologies: predicting alcohol related problems in college drinking men and women. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 167, 57–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Locey, M. L., Jones, B. A., & Rachlin, H. (2011). Real and hypothetical rewards in self-control and social discounting. Judgment and Decision making, 6(6), 522–564.

    Google Scholar 

  • Madden, G. J., Begotka, A. M., Raiff, B. R., & Kastern, L. L. (2003). Delay discounting of real and hypothetical rewards. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 11(2), 139–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Madden, G. J., & Bickel, W. K. (2010). Impulsivity: the behavioral and neurological science of discounting. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mobini, S., Grant, A., Kass, A. E., & Yeomans, M. R. (2007). Relationships between functional and dysfunctional impulsivity, delay discounting and cognitive distortions. Personality and Individual Differences, 43(6), 1517–1528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Myerson, J., Green, L., & Warusawitharana, M. (2001). Area under the curve as a measure of discounting. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 76, 235–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nederkoorn, C., Smulders, F. T. Y., Havermans, R. C., Roefs, A., & Jansen, A. (2006). Impulsivity in obese women. Appetite, 47(2), 253–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nooyens, A. C. J., Visscher, T. L. S., Verschuren, W. M., Schuit, A. J., Boshuizen, H. C., van Mechelen, W., & Seidell, J. C. (2009). Age, period and cohort effects on body weight and body mass index in adults: the doetinchem cohort study. Public Health Nutrition, 12(6), 862–870.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nyholm, M., Gullberg, B., Merlo, J., Lundqvist-Persson, C., Råstam, L., & Lindblad, U. (2007). The validity of obesity based on self-reported weight and height: implications for population studies. Obesity, 15(1), 197–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, E. A., Rosso, I. M., Demers, L. A., Divatia, S., & Killgore, W. D. S. (2016). Sex differences in psychological factors associated with social discounting. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 29(1), 60–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rachlin, H., & Raineri, A. (1992). Irrationality, impulsiveness, and selfishness as discount reversal effects. In G. Loewenstein & J. Elster (Eds.), Choice over time (pp. 93–118). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rachlin, H., & Jones, B. A. (2008a). Altruism among relatives and non-relatives. Behavioural Processes, 79, 120–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rachlin, H., & Jones, B. A. (2008b). Social discounting and delay discounting. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 21, 29–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reas, D. L., Nygård, J. F., Svensson, E., Sørensen, T., & Sandanger, I. (2007). Changes in body mass index by age, gender, and socio-economic status among a cohort of Norwegian men and women (1990-2001). BMC Public Health, 7, 269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reimers, S., Maylor, E. A., Stewart, N., & Chater, N. (2009). Associations between a one-shot delay discounting measure and age, income, education and real-world impulsive behavior. Personality and Individual Differences, 47(8), 973–978.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Romanowich, P., & Igaki, T. (2017). Effects of reward magnitude, tobacco, and alcohol use on social discounting in Japan and United States college students. The Psychological Record, 67, 345–353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rozin, P. (2005). The meaning of food in our lives: a cross-cultural perspective on eating and well-being. Journal of Nutrition Education and Behavior, 37, S107–S112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, D. J. (2012). The developing mind: how relationships and the brain interact to shape who we are. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silverman, I. W. (2003). Gender differences in delay of gratification: a meta- analysis. Sex Roles, 49(9), 451–463.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steinberg, L., Graham, S., O'Brien, L., Woolard, J., Cauffman, E., & Banich, M. (2009). Age differences in future orientation and delay discounting. Child Development, 80(1), 28–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thamotharan, S., Lange, K., Ramos, A., & Fields, S. (2016). Examining weight concern and delay discounting in adolescent females. Eating Behaviors, 21, 228–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wainwright, K., & Romanowich, P. (2016). The relationship between perfectionism and discounting measures. Personality and Individual Differences, 101, 407–412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weafer, J., & de Wit, H. (2014). Sex differences in impulsive action and impulsive choice. Addictive Behaviors, 39(11), 1573–1579.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weafer, J., De Arcangelis, J., & de Wit, H. (2015). Sex differences in behavioral impulsivity in at-risk and non-risk drinkers. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 6, 7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weatherly, J. N., & McDonald, J. D. (2011). Delay discounting on different outcomes in a sample of American Indian and non-Indian college students. American Indian and Alaska Native Mental Health Research (Online), 17(2), 43–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weller, R. E., Cook III, E. W., Avsar, K. B., & Cox, J. E. (2008). Obese women show greater delay discounting than healthy-weight women. Appetite, 51(3), 563–569.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whelan, R., & McHugh, L. A. (2009). Temporal discounting of hypothetical monetary rewards by adolescents, adults, and older adults. The Psychological Record, 59(2), 247–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • World Health Organization. (2017a). BMI classification. Retrieved from http://apps.who.int/bmi/index.jsp?introPage=intro_3.html

  • World Health Organization. (2017b). BMI-for-age (5–19 years). Retrieved from http://www.who.int/growthref/who2007_bmi_for_age/en/

  • Yeomans, M. R., Leitch, M., & Mobini, S. (2008). Impulsivity is associated with the disinhibition but not restraint factor from the three-factor eating questionnaire. Appetite, 50(2–3), 469–476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yi, R., Johnson, M. W., Giordano, L. A., Landes, R. D., Badger, G. J., & Bickel, W. K. (2008). The effects of reduced cigarette smoking on discounting future rewards: an initial evaluation. The Psychological Record, 58(2), 163–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paul Romanowich.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Wainwright, K., Green, B.E. & Romanowich, P. The Relationship Between Delay and Social Discounting, and Body Mass Index in University Students. Psychol Rec 68, 441–449 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-018-0287-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-018-0287-y

Keywords

Navigation