Skip to main content
Log in

Exploring Differential Trial-Type Effects and the Impact of a Read-Aloud Procedure on Deictic Relational Responding on the IRAP

  • Original Article
  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Under the rubric of Relational Frame Theory (RFT), researchers have investigated the role of deictic relational responding in the analysis of self in relation to others, place, and time, primarily through the use of an extended developmental protocol (Barnes-Holmes, 2001). In a move toward extending methodologies for studying deictic relational responding, more recent research has employed the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) to measure deictic relational responding regarding I versus OTHER (Barbero-Rubio et al. in The Psychological Record, 66, 243–252, 2016). The initial purpose of the current study was to systematically replicate and extend this research. This extension involved the inclusion of a control condition in which no responding to self was involved, only responding to others. The results from Experiment 1 yielded significant IRAP effects for two of the four trial-types in both the deictic and control IRAPs. A second experiment involved a novel method for collecting IRAP data (a read-aloud procedure), which had been shown to yield significant effects for all four trial-types, and four significant effects were indeed recorded for both deictic and control IRAPs. Based on the current findings, a model is presented that seeks to explain the differential trial-type effects that are observed across the different IRAPs and the impact of the read-aloud procedure.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. It is important to emphasize that the current research was a systematic, rather than direct, replication of Barbero-Rubio et al. (2016), in that there were numerous procedural differences between the earlier work and the present research (see Sidman, 1960).

  2. We subjected the raw reaction time data from this IRAP to the same analyses as those conducted by Barbero-Rubio et al. (2016) and found broadly similar results for the trial types that produced significant DIRAP-scores in our study, but not for the trial types that produced non-significant effects.

References

  • Alexopoulos, T., Muller, D., Ric, F., & Marendaz, C. (2012). I, me, mine: Automatic attentional capture by self-related stimuli. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42, 770–779.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barbero-Rubio, A., Lopez-Lopez, J., Luciano, C., & Eisenbeck, N. (2016). Perspective-taking measured by implicit relational assessment procedure (IRAP). The Psychological Record, 66, 243–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnes-Holmes, Y. (2001). Analysing relational frames: Studying language and cognition in young children (unpublished doctoral thesis). National University of Ireland Maynooth.

  • Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Stewart, I., & Boles, S. (2010). A sketch of the implicit relational assessment procedure (IRAP) and the relational elaboration and coherence (REC) model. The Psychological Record, 60, 527–542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnes-Holmes, D., Finn, M., McEnteggart, C., & Barnes-Holmes, Y. (2017). Derived stimulus relations and their roles in a behavior-analytic account of human language and cognition. The Behavior Analyst. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40614-017-0124-7.

  • Davis, M. H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10, 85.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Corte, K., Buysee, A., Verhofstadt, L., Roeyers, H., Ponnet, K., & Davis, M. (2007). Measuring empathetic tendencies: Reliability and validity of the Dutch version of the interpersonal reactivity index (IRI). Psychologica Belgica, 47, 235–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finn, M., Barnes-Holmes, D., Hussey, I., & Graddy, J. (2016). Exploring the behavioral dynamics of the implicit relational assessment procedure: The impact of three types of introductory rules. The Psychological Record, 66, 309–321

  • Finn, M., Barnes-Holmes, & McEnteggart, C. (2017). Exploring the single-trial-type-dominance-effect in the IRAP: Developing a differential arbitrarily applicable relational responding effects (DAARRE) model. The Psychological Record. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-017-0262-z.

  • Franck, E., De Raedt, R., Barbez, C., & Rosseel, Y. (2008). Psychometric properties of the Dutch Rosenberg self-esteem scale. Psychologica Belgica, 48, 25–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gore, N. G., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Murphy, G. (2010). The relationship between intellectual functioning and relational perspective-taking. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 10, 1–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2001). Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York: Kluwer Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heagle, A. I., & Rehfeldt, R. A. (2006). Teaching perspective-taking skills to typically developing children through derived relational responding. Journal of Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention, 3, 1–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hussey, I., McEnteggart, C., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Kavanagh, D., Barnes-Holmes, D., Parling, T., & Lundgren, T. (2014). Flexible perspective-taking: New concepts and a new behavioural measure. Dublin: ACT CBS Conference.

    Google Scholar 

  • Konings, M., Bak, M., Hanssen, M., Van Os, J., & Krabbendam, L. (2006). Validity and reliability of the CAPE: A self-report instrument for the measurement of psychotic experiences in the general population. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 114, 55–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maloney, E., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2016). Exploring the behavioral dynamics of the implicit relational assessment procedure: The role of relational contextual cues versus relational coherence indicators as response options. The Psychological Record, 66, 395–403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McHugh, L., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2004). Perspective-taking as relational responding: A developmental profile. The Psychological Record, 54, 115–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robins, R. W., Hendin, H. M., & Trzesniewski, K. H. (2001). Measuring global self-esteem: Construct validation of a single-item measure and the Rosenberg self-esteem scale. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 151–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (1960). Tactics of scientific research: Evaluating experimental in psychology. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stefanis, N. C., Hanssen, M., Smirnis, N. K., Avramopoulos, D. A., Evdokimidis, I. K., Stefanis, C. N., Verdoux, H., & Van Os, J. (2002). Evidence that three dimensions of psychosis have a distribution in the general population. Psychological Medicine, 32, 347–358.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vahey, N. A., Nicholson, E., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2015). A meta-analysis of criterion effects for the implicit relational assessment procedure (IRAP) in the clinical domain. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 48, 59–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Villatte, M., Monestés, J.-L., McHugh, L., Freixa i Baqué, E. F. i., & Loas, G. (2008). Assessing deictic relational responding in social anhedonia: A functional approach to the development of theory of mind impairments. Journal of Behavioral Consultation and Therapy, 4, 360–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Villatte, M., Monestés, J.-L., McHugh, L., Freixa i Baqué, E., & Loas, G. (2010). Assessing perspective taking in schizophrenia using relational frame theory. The Psychological Record, 60, 413–436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vitale, A., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Campbell, C. (2008). Facilitating responding in accordance with the relational frame of comparison: Systematic empirical analyses. The Psychological Record, 58, 365–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weil, T. M., Hayes, S. C., & Capurro, P. (2011). Establishing a deictic relational repertoire in young children. The Psychological Record, 61, 371–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This research was conducted with funding from the FWO Type I Odysseus Program at Ghent University, Belgium awarded to Dermot Barnes-Holmes.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Deirdre Kavanagh.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures in the current study were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional research committee, and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants.

Additional information

The data for the current manuscript was collected and prepared with the support of the FWO Type I Odysseus Program at Ghent University, Belgium. We are very grateful to the reviewers for their thorough and constructive commentaries.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kavanagh, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D. et al. Exploring Differential Trial-Type Effects and the Impact of a Read-Aloud Procedure on Deictic Relational Responding on the IRAP. Psychol Rec 68, 163–176 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-018-0276-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-018-0276-1

Keywords

Navigation