Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Teaching in Hunter-Gatherers

  • Published:
Review of Philosophy and Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Most of what we know about teaching comes from research among people living in large, politically and economically stratified societies with formal education systems and highly specialized roles with a global market economy. In this paper, we review and synthesize research on teaching among contemporary hunter-gatherer societies. The hunter-gatherer lifeway is the oldest humanity has known and is more representative of the circumstances under which teaching evolved and was utilized most often throughout human history. Research among contemporary hunter-gatherers also illustrates a complex pattern of teaching that is both consistent with and distinct from teaching in other small- and large-scale societies with different subsistence practices and cultural forms. In particular, we find that the cultural emphasis on individual autonomy and socio-political egalitarianism among hunter-gatherers differently shapes how teaching occurs. For example, teaching clearly exists among hunter-gatherers and appears in many forms, including institutionalized instruction in valued cultural and technical skills. However, teaching tends to be less common in hunter-gatherer societies because people live in small, intimate egalitarian, groups that support each other’s learning in a variety of ways without teaching. Furthermore, foundational cultural schemas of autonomy and egalitarianism impact the nature of teaching. For example, adults and older children limit their interventions, permitting autonomous learning, and, when they occur, teaching episodes are generally brief, subtle, indirect, and situated in a present activity (i.e. knowledge is not objectified or intended to be generalizable). We discuss the implications of this research in terms of discussions of the evolution of human cognition and the co-evolution of teaching and culture through the process of cultural niche construction.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Lewis (2016) also describes music as a foundational schema among Congo Basin hunter-gatherers, emphasizing its unique role in constructing hunter-gatherer cultural uniqueness. We will not discuss music here specifically as a foundational schema but will review his work below. Here we are concerned with pan-hunter-gatherer schemas.

References

  • Bird, D.W., and R. Bliege Bird. 2005. Martu children’s hunting strategies in the Western Desert, Australia. In Hunter-gatherer childhoods: Evolutionary, developmental, and cultural perspectives, ed. B.S. Hewlett and M.E. Lamb, 129–146. New Brunswick: Aldine Transaction.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bird-David, N. 1990. The giving environment: Another perspective on the economic system of gatherer-hunters. Current Anthropology 31 (2): 189–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bird-David, N. 1992. Beyond “the hunting and gathering mode of subsistence”:Culture-senstive observations on the Nayaka and other modern hunter-gatherers. Man 27 (1): 19–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bird-David, N. 1999. “Animism” revisited: Personhood, environment, and relational epistemology. Current Anthropology 40 (S1): S67–S91. doi:10.1086/200061.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bliege Bird, R., and D.W. Bird. 2002. Constraints of knowing or constraints of growing? Fishing and collecting by the children of Mer. Human Nature 13 (2): 239–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blurton Jones, N., K. Hawkes, and J.F. O’Connell. 1989. Modelling and measuring costs of children in two foraging societies. In Comparative Socioecology: The Behavioural ecology of humans and other mammals, ed. V. Standen and R. Foley, 367–390. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bock, J. 2002. Learning, life history, and productivity: children’s lives in the Okavango Delta, Botswana. Human Nature 13 (2): 161–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bourdieu, P. 1977. Outline of a theory of practice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • Boyd, R., P.J. Richerson, & J. Henrich. 2011. Colloquium paper: The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108 (Supplement 2), 10918–10925. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1100290108.

  • Boyette, A.H. 2013. Social learning during middle childhood among Aka foragers and Ngandu farmers of the Central African Republic. PhD Dissertation. Washington State University, Pullman, WA.

  • Boyette, A.H. 2016a. Children’s play and culture learning in an egalitarian foraging society. Child Development 87 (3): 759–769. doi:10.1111/cdev.12496.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyette, A.H. 2016b. Children’s play and the integration of social and individual learning: A cultural niche construction perspective. In Social learning and Innovation in contemporary hunter-gatherers, ed. H. Terashima and B.S. Hewlett, 159–169. Tokyo: Springer Japan Retrieved from http://link.springer.com/10.1007/978-4-431-55997-9_13.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Boyette, A. H., & Hewlett, B. S. 2017. Autonomy, equality, and teaching among aka foragers and Ngandu farmers of the Congo Basin. Human Nature. doi:10.1007/s12110-017-9294-y

  • Briggs, J.L. 1991. Expecting the unexpected: Canadian Inuit training for an experimental lifestyle. Ethos 19 (3): 259–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caro, T.M., and M.D. Hauser. 1992. Is there teaching in nonhuman animals? The Quarterly Review of Biology 67 (2): 151–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Castro, L., and M.A. Toro. 2004. The evolution of culture: From primate social learning to human culture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 101 (27): 10235–10240. doi:10.1073/pnas.0400156101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crittenden, A. N. 2016. Children’s foraging and play among the Hadza: The evolutionary significance of “work play.” In Childhood: Origins, Evolution, and Implications, ed. C. Meehan and A. Crittenden, 155–171. Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Press.

  • Csibra, G., and G. Gergely. 2009. Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13 (4): 148–153. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csibra, G., and G. Gergely. 2011. Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 366 (1567): 1149–1157. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • D’Andrade, R.G. 1992. Schemas and motivation. In Human motives and cultural models, ed. R.G. D’Andrade and C. Strauss, 23–44. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Dira, S.J., and B.S. Hewlett. 2016. Learning to spear hunt among Ethiopian Chabu adolescent hunter-gatherers. In Social learning and Innovation in contemporary hunter-gatherers: Evolutionary and ethnographic perspectives, eds. H. Terashima and B.S. Hewlett, 71–82. Tokyo: Springer.

  • Draper, P. 1973. Crowding among hunter-gatherers: The !Kung bushmen. Science 182 (4109): 301–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Endicott, K.M. 2011. Cooperative autonomy: Social solidarity among the Batek of Malaysia. In Anarchic solidarity: Autonomy, equality, and fellowship in Southeast Asia, ed. T. Gibson and K. Sillander, 62–87. New Haven: Yake University Council on Southeast Asia Studies.

    Google Scholar 

  • Endicott, K.L., and K.M. Endicott. 2014. Batek childrearing and morality. In Ancestral Landscapes in Human Evolution (pp. 108–125), ed. D. Narvaez, K. Valentino, A. Fuentes, J.J. McKenna, and P. Gray. Oxford University Press Retrieved from http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/view/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199964253.001.0001/acprof-9780199964253-chapter-8.

  • Enquist, M., S. Ghirlanda, and K. Eriksson. 2011. Modelling the evolution and diversity of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 366 (1563): 412–423. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Firth, R. 1970. Education in Tikopia. In From child to adult: Studies in the anthropology of education, 75–90. Garden City: The Natural History Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fogarty, L., P. Strimling, and K.N. Laland. 2011. The evolution of teaching. Evolution 65 (10): 2760–2770.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fortes, M. 1970. Social and psychological aspects of education in Taleland. In From child to adult: Studies in the anthropology of education, ed. J. Middleton, 14–74. Garden City: The Natural History Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Franks, N.R., and T. Richardson. 2006. Teaching in tandem-running ants. Nature 439 (7073): 153–153. doi:10.1038/439153a.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, P. 1991. Foragers’ pursuit of individual autonomy. Current Anthropology 32: 543–558.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garfield, Z., M. Garfield, and B.S. Hewlett. 2016. A cross-cultural analysis of hunter-gatherer social learning. In Social learning and Innovation in contemporary hunter-gatherers: Evolutionary and ethnographic perspectives, ed. H. Terashima and B.S. Hewlett, 19–34. Tokyo: Springer Japan.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gergely, G., and G. Csibra. 2006. Sylvia’s receipe: The role of imitation and pedagogy in the transmission of human culture. In Roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition and human interaction, ed. N.J. Enfield and S.C. Levenson, 229–255. Oxford: Berg Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, N.S., S. Burke, H. Akil, S.J. Watson, and J. Panksepp. 2003. Socially-induced brain “fertilization”: Play promotes brain derived neurotrophic factor transcription in the amygdala and dorsolateral frontal cortex in juvenile rats. Neuroscience Letters 341 (1): 17–20. doi:10.1016/S0304-3940(03)00158-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenfield, P. M. (2004). Weaving generations together: evolving creativity in the Maya of Chiapas. Santa Fe, N.M: School of American Research Press.

  • Guemple, L. 1979. Inuit socialization: A study of children as social actors in an Eskimo community. In Childhood and adolescence in Canada, ed. K. Ishwaran, 39–53. Toronto: McGraw Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guemple, L. 1988. Teaching social realtions to Inuit children. In Hunters and gatherers 2: Property, power, and ideology, ed. T. Ingold, D. Riches, and J. Woodburn, 131–149. Oxford: Berg Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gurven, M. 2004. To give and to give not: The behavioral ecology of human food transfers. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27: 543–583.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gurven, M., H. Kaplan, and M. Gutierrez. 2006. How long does it take to become a proficient hunter? Implications for the evolution of extended development and long life span. Journal of Human Evolution 51 (5): 454–470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, P.L., and K.H. Corriveau. 2011. Young children’s selective trust in informants. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 366 (1567): 1179–1187. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hart, C.W.M., and A.R. Pilling. 1960. The Tiwi of North Australia. New York: Holt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., and R. McElreath. 2003. The evolution of cultural evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews 12 (3): 123–135. doi:10.1002/evan.10110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett, B.S. 1991. Intimate fathers : The nature and context of aka pygmy paternal infant care. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett, B.L. 2013. Listen, here is a story: Ethnographic life narratives from aka and Ngandu women of the Congo basin. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett, B.S. 2015. Chabu forager-farmer perceptions of social learning. Paper presented at International Conference on Teacher’s Cognition, Paris, France.

  • Hewlett, B.S., and L.L. Cavalli-Sforza. 1986. Cultural transmission among Aka pygmies. American Anthropologist 88: 922–933.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett, B.S., and J.W. Roulette. 2014. Cosleeping beyond infancy. In Ancestral Landscapes in Human Evolution: Culture, Childrearing and Social Wellbeing, ed. D. Narvaez, K. Valentino, A. Fuentes, J. McKenna, P. Gray, 129–163. New York: Oxford University Press.

  • Hewlett, B.S., and C.J. Roulette. 2016. Teaching in hunter–gatherer infancy. Royal Society Open Science 3 (1): 150403. doi:10.1098/rsos.150403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett, B.S., M.E. Lamb, D. Shannon, B. Leyendecker, and A. Schölmerich. 1998. Culture and early infancy among central African foragers and farmers. Developmental Psychology 34 (4): 653–661.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett, B.S., M.E. Lamb, B. Leyendecker, and A. Schölmerich. 2000. Internal working models, trust and sharing among foragers. Current Anthropology 41 (2): 287–297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett, B.S., H.N. Fouts, A.H. Boyette, and B.L. Hewlett. 2011. Social learning among Congo Basin hunter-gatherers. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 366: 1168–1178. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, K., M. Barton, and A.M. Hurtado. 2009. The emergence of human uniqueness: Characters underlying behavioral modernity. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews 18 (5): 187–200. doi:10.1002/evan.20224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, K.R., B.M. Wood, J. Baggio, A.M. Hurtado, and R.T. Boyd. 2014. Hunter-gatherer inter-band interaction rates: Implications for cumulative culture. PloS One 9 (7): e102806. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0102806.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kline, M.A. 2015. How to learn about teaching: An evolutionary framework for the study of teaching behavior in humans and other animals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences: 1–70. doi:10.1017/S0140525X14000090.

  • Kline, M. A. (2016). TEACH: An ethogram-based method to observe and record teaching behavior. Field Methods doi:10.1177/1525822X16669282.

  • Knutson, B., J. Burgdorf, and J. Panksepp. 1998. Anticipation of play elicits high-frequency ultrasonic vocalizations in young rats. Journal of Comparative Psychology 112 (1): 65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Konner, M. 2005. Hunter-gatherer infancy and childhood: The !Kung and others. In Hunter-gatherer childhoods: Evolutionary, developmental, and cultural perspectives, ed. B.S. Hewlett and M.E. Lamb, 19–64. New Brunswick: Aldine Transaction.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruger, A.C., and M. Tomasello. 1996. Cultural learning and learning culture. In The handbook of human development and education, ed. D. Olson and N. Torrance, 369–387. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K.N. 2017. Darwin’s unfinished symphony: How culture explains the evolution of the human mind. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lancy, D.F. 2010. Learning “from nobody”: The limited role of teaching in folk models of children’s development. Childhood in the Past 3: 79–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lancy, D.F. 2012. Ethnographic perspectives on cultural transmission/acquisition. In Paper prepared for the School of American Research. Santa Fe, Andvanced Seminar: Multiple perspectives on the evolution of childhood.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lancy, D.F. 2016. Teaching: Natural or cultural? In Evolutionary perspectives on education and child development, ed. D. Berch and D. Geary. Heidelberg: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • Lee, R. B. 1969. Eating Christmas in the Kalahari. Conformity and Conflict: Readings in Cultural Anthropology, 27–34.

  • Lee, R. B. (2013). The Dobe Ju/‘hoansi (4th ed, Student ed). Belmont: Wadsworth Cengage Learning.

  • Lewis, J. 2002. Forest hunter-gatherers and their world: A study of the Mbendjele Yaka Pygmies of Congo-Brazzaville and their secular and religious activites and representations. (doctoral thesis). London School of Economics and Political Science.

  • Lewis, J. 2009. As well as words: Congo pygmy hunting, mimicry, and play. In The cradle of language, ed. R. Botha and C. Knight, 236–256. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, J. 2015. Where goods are free but knowledge costs: Hunter-gatherer ritual economics in Western Central Africa. Hunter Gatherer Research 1 (1): 1–27. doi:10.3828/hgr.2015.2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, J. 2016. Play music and taboo in the reproduction of an egalitarian society. In Social learning and Innovation in contemporary hunter-gatherers: Evolutionary and ethnographic perspectives, ed. B.S. Hewlett and H. Terashima, 147–158. Tokyo: Springer Japan.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, H.M., L. Vinicius, J. Strods, R. Mace, and A.B. Migliano. 2014. High mobility explains demand sharing and enforced cooperation in egalitarian hunter-gatherers. Nature Communications 5: 5789. doi:10.1038/ncomms6789.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lothrop, S.K. 1978. The Indians of Tierra del Fuego. (1st AMS ed) ed. New York: AMS Press.

  • Lye, T.-P. 2004. Changing pathways: Forest degradation and the Batek of Pahang, Malaysia. Lanham: Lexington Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald, K. 2007. Cross-cultural comparison of learning in human hunting. Human Nature 18 (4): 386–402. doi:10.1007/s12110-007-9019-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald, D.H., and B.S. Hewlett. 1999. Reproductive interests and forager mobility. Current Anthropology 40 (4): 501–523.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marshall, J. 1958. Man the hunter. Natural History 67: 291–309.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maynard, A.E. 2002. Cultural teaching: The development of teaching skills in Maya sibling interactions. Child Development 73 (3): 969–982. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mead, M. 1928. Coming of age in Samoa: A psychological study of primitive youth for Western civilization. New York: Morrow Quill Paperbacks.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mead, M. 1964. Continuities in cultural evolution. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mead, M. 1970. Our educational emphases in primitive perspective. In From child to adult: Studies in the anthropology of education, ed. J. Middleton, 1–13. Garden City: The Natural History Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meehan, C.L. 2005. The effects of residential locality on parental and alloparental investment among the aka foragers of the central African Republic. Human Nature 16 (1): 58–80. doi:10.1007/s12110-005-1007-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Menary, R. 2014. Neural plasticity, neuronal recycling, and niche construction. Mind and Language 29 (3): 286–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, R. 2017. Pedagogy and social learning in human development. In The Routledge Handbook of the Social Mind, ed. J. Kiverstein, 35–52. New York: Routeldge.

  • Morgan, T.J.H., N.T. Uomini, L.E. Rendell, L. Chouinard-Thuly, S.E. Street, H.M. Lewis, et al. 2015. Experimental evidence for the co-evolution of hominin tool-making teaching and language. Nature Communications 6: 6029. doi:10.1038/ncomms7029.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Myers, F. 1986. Pintupi country, Pintupi self: Sentiment, place and politics among Western Desert aborigines. Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian Institution Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Naveh, D. 2016. Social and epistemological dimensions of learning among Nayaka hunter-gatherers. In Social learning and Innovation in contemporary hunter-gatherers: Evolutionary and ethnographic perspectives, eds. H. Terashima and B.S. Hewlett, 125–134. Tokyo: Springer Japan.

  • Omura, K. 2016. Socio-cultural cultivation of positive attitudes toward learning: Considering differences in learning ability between Neanderhals and modern humans from examining Inuit children’s learning process. In Social learning and Innovation in contemporary hunter-gatherers: Evolutionary and ethnographic perspectives, ed. B.S. Hewlett and H. Terashima, 267–284. Tokyo: Springer Japan.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, N. 1993. Demand sharing: Reciprocity and the pressure for generosity among foragers. American Anthropologist 95 (4): 860–874.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raihani, N.J., and A.R. Ridley. 2008. Experimental evidence for teaching in wild pied babblers. Animal Behaviour 75: 3–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B. 2011. Childhood and learning: How do chidren learn without being taught? One way is by observing and pitching in. Anthropology of Childhood and Youth Interest Group Newsletter, (October).

  • Rogoff, B., R. Paradise, R.M. Arauz, M. Correa-Chávez, and C. Angelillo. 2003. Firsthand learning through intent participation. Annual Review of Psychology 54 (1): 175–203. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scalise Sugiyama, M. 2017. Oral storytelling as evidence of pedagogy in forager societies. Frontiers in Psychology 8. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00471.

  • Schlegel, A., and H. Barry. 1991. Adolescence: An Anthropological inquiry. NY: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schnegg, M. 2015. Reciprocity on demand: Sharing and exchanging food in northwestern Namibia. Human Nature 26 (3): 313–330. doi:10.1007/s12110-015-9236-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shennan, S.J., and J. Steele. 1999. Cultural learning in hominids: A behavioral ecological apporach. In Mammalian social learning: Comparative and ecological perspectives, ed. H.O. Box and J.R. Gibson, 367–388. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shore, B. 1996. Culture in mind: Cognition, culture, and the problem of meaning. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silberbauer, G. B. (1981). Hunter and habitat in the central Kalahari Desert. Cambridge [Eng.]; New York: Cambridge University press.

  • Sterelny, K. 2007. Social intelligence, human intelligence and niche construction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 362 (1480): 719–730. doi:10.1098/rstb.2006.2006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny, K. 2012a. Language, gesture, skill: The co-evolutionary foundations of language. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 367 (1599): 2141–2151. doi:10.1098/rstb.2012.0116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny, K. 2012b. The evolved apprentice: How evolution made humans unique. Cambridge: The MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Strauss, S., and M. Ziv. 2012. Teaching is a natural cognitive ability for humans. Mind, Brain, and Education 6 (4): 186–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strauss, S., M. Ziv, and A. Stein. 2002. Teaching as a natural cognition and its relations to preschoolers’ developing theory of mind. Cognitive Development 17: 1473–1787.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sugawara, K. 1984. Spatial proximity and bodily contact among the central Kalahari san. African Study Monographs 3: 1–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Takada, A. 2016. Education and learning during social situations among the central Kalahari san. In Social learning and Innovation in contemporary hunter-gatherers: Evolutionary and ethnographic perspectives, ed. B.S. Hewlett and H. Terashima, 97–112. Tokyo: Springer Japan.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tehrani, J.J., and M. Collard. 2009. On the relationship between interindividual cultural transmission and population-level cultural diversity: A case study of weaving in Iranian tribal populations. Evolution and Human Behavior 30: 286–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thornton, A., and K. McAuliffe. 2006. Teaching in Wild Meerkats. Science 313 (5784): 227–229. doi:10.1126/science.1128727.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thornton, A., and N.J. Raihani. 2008. The evolution of teaching. Animal Behaviour 75 (6): 1823–1836.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., A.C. Kruger, and H.H. Ratner. 1993. Culture learning. 16: 495–552.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., M. Carpenter, J. Call, T. Behne, and H. Moll. 2005. Understanding and sharing intentions: The origions of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28: 675–735.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A., and D. Erdal. 2012. The human socio-cognitive niche and its evolutionary origins. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 367 (1599): 2119–2129. doi:10.1098/rstb.2012.0114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A., V. Horner, and S. Marchall-Pescini. 2003. Cultural panthropology. Evolutionary Anthropology 12: 92–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiessner, P. 1982. Risk, reciprocity, and social influences on !Kung san economics. In Politics and history in band societies, ed. E. Leacock and R.B. Lee, 61–84. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiessner, P. 2002. Hunting, healing, and hxaro exchange: A long-term perspective on !Kung (Ju/‘hoansi) large-game hunting. Evolution and Human Behavior 23: 407–436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodburn, J. 1982. Egalitarian societies. Man 17 (3): 431–451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Adam H. Boyette.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Boyette, A.H., Hewlett, B.S. Teaching in Hunter-Gatherers. Rev.Phil.Psych. 9, 771–797 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-017-0347-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-017-0347-2

Navigation