Skip to main content
Log in

The Relationship Between Decentering and Adaptiveness of Response Styles in Reducing Depression

  • ORIGINAL PAPER
  • Published:
Mindfulness Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Recent studies have suggested that ruminative and distractive response styles to negative moods could have adaptive and maladaptive consequences, respectively. However, whether there is a common component in the adaptive aspects of ruminative or distractive response styles has not been examined sufficiently. Adaptiveness of ruminative or distractive response styles is considered to be associated with decentering; thus, we investigated the indirect effects of four types of the response style (adaptive/maladaptive aspects of each response style) on depression through decentering. In this study, 241 Japanese undergraduate students (75 men and 166 women) were assessed using the Response Style Scale, the Japanese Version of Experience Questionnaire, and the Japanese Version of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D). Mediation analyses revealed that decentering fully mediated the effects of adaptive aspects of ruminative and distractive response styles on depression whereas it partially mediated the effects of maladaptive aspects of response styles. These results indicate that the adaptiveness of response styles is influenced by decentering, and it particularly works in the adaptive aspects of ruminative/distractive response styles as a common element. Thus, a possibility is suggested that cultivating decentered perspective is effective in reducing maladaptive aspects of both response styles to negative moods or objects.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173–1182.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., Carmody, J., et al. (2004). Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 11(3), 230–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blagden, J. C., & Craske, M. G. (1996). Effects of active and passive rumination and distraction: A pilot replication with anxious mood. Journal of Anxiety Disorder, 10(4), 243–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Broderick, P. C. (2005). Mindfulness and coping with dysphoric mood: Contrasts with rumination and distraction. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 29(5), 501–510.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ciarocco, N. J., Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2010). Some good news about rumination: Task-focused thinking after failure facilitates performance improvement. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 29(10), 1057–1073.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, K. M., Lau, M. A., & Cairns, D. R. (2009). Development and preliminary validation of a trait version of the Toronto Mindfulness Scale. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 23(3), 185–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fresco, D. M., Moore, M. T., van Dulmen, M. H. M., Segal, Z. V., Ma, S. H., Teasdale, J. D., et al. (2007). Initial psychometric properties of the Experiences Questionnaire: Validation of a self-report measure of decentering. Behavior Therapy, 38(3), 234–246.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hattori, Y., & Kawaguchi, J. (2010). Decreased effectiveness of a focused–distraction strategy in dysphoric individuals. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24(3), 376–386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ito, T., Takenaka, K., Tomita, T., & Agari, I. (2006). Comparison of ruminative responses with negative rumination as a vulnerability factor for depression. Psychological Reports, 99(3), 763–772.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jain, S., Shapiro, S. L., Swanick, S., Roesch, S. C., Mills, P. J., Bell, I., et al. (2007). A randomized controlled trial of mindfulness meditation versus relaxation training: Effects on distress, positive states of mind, rumination, and distraction. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 33(1), 11–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Joormann, J., Dkane, M., & Gotlib, I. H. (2006). Adaptive and maladaptive components of rumination? Behavior Therapy, 37(3), 269–280.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: Past, present, and future. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10(2), 144–156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kodama, H., Katayanagi, K., Shimada, H., & Sakano, Y. (1994). The relationship among stress coping with automatic thoughts, state anxiety, and depressive symptoms in college students. Waseda Human Science Research, 7, 14–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kurihara, I., Hasegawa, A., & Nedate, K. (2010). Development of the Japanese version of the Experiences Questionnaire and examination of its reliability and validity. The Japanese Journal of Personality, 19(2), 174–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lau, M. A., Bishop, S. R., Segal, Z. V., Buis, T., Anderson, N. D., Carlson, L., et al. (2006). The Toronto Mindfulness Scale: Development and validation. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62(12), 1445–1467.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lin, Y. J., & Wicker, F. W. (2007). A comparison of the effects of thought suppression, distraction and concentration. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(12), 2924–2937.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lyubomirsky, S., Kasri, F., & Zehm, K. (2003). Dysphoric rumination impairs concentration on academic tasks. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(3), 309–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyubomirsky, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1995). Effects of self-focused rumination on negative thinking and interpersonal problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(1), 176–190.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lyubomirsky, S., Tucker, K. L., Caldwell, N. D., & Berg, K. (1999). Why ruminators are poor problem solvers: Clues from the phenomenology of dysphoric rumination. Journal of Personality and Psychology, 77(5), 1041–1060.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matsumoto, M. (2008). Development of Expanded Response Styles Questionnaire. The Japanese Journal of Personality, 16(2), 209–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matsumoto, M. (2013). Influences of rumination, meta-mood, and decentering on depression: A survey of university students and vocational school students. Japanese Journal of Applied Psychology, 38(3), 211–221.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mor, N., & Winquist, J. (2002). Self-focused attention and negative affect: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128(4), 638–662.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morrow, J., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1990). Effects of responses to depression on the remediation of depressive affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(3), 519–527.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100(4), 569–582.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1991). A prospective study of depression and posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster: The 1989 Loma Prieta earthquake. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(1), 115–121.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1993). Effects of rumination and distraction on naturally occurring depressed mood. Cognition and Emotion, 7(6), 561–570.

  • Oikawa, M. (2002). Distraction as an intervention for depression: The distraction process. Japanese Journal of Educational Psychology, 50(2), 185–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oikawa, M. (2003). Self-regulation of mood through distraction: The distraction process. Japanese Journal of Educational Psychology, 51(4), 443–456.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramel, W., Goldin, P. R., Carmona, P. E., & McQuaid, J. R. (2004). The effects of mindfulness meditation on cognitive processes and affect in patients with past depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 28(4), 433–455.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rude, S. S., Maestas, K. L., & Neff, K. (2007). Paying attention to distress: What’s wrong with rumination? Cognition & Emotion, 21(4), 843–864.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Segerstrom, S. C., Stanton, A. L., Alden, L. E., & Shortridge, B. E. (2003). A multidimensional structure for repetitive thought: What’s on your mind, and how, and how much? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(5), 909–921.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, S. L., Brown, K. W., & Biegel, G. M. (2007). Teaching self-care to caregivers: Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction on the mental health of therapists in training. Training and Education in Professional Psychology, 1(2), 105–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shima, S., Shikano, T., Kitamura, T., & Asai, M. (1985). New self-rating scales for depression. Clinical Psychiatry, 27(6), 717–723.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shimazu, N. (2010). Construction of a new response style scale and examination of its reliability and validity. The scientific researches. Educational psychology, 58, 29–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shimazu, N., & Koshikawa, F. (2014). Study of response styles and a causal model of depression. The Japanese Journal of Psychology, 85(4), 392–397.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shimazu, N., Koshikawa, F., & Kondo, I. (2008). Developing a new response styles scale (RSS). In T. P. S. Oei & C. S. Tang (Eds.), Current research & practices on cognitive behaviour therapy in Asia (pp. 8–15). Brisbane: CBT Unit Toowong Private Hospital.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steil, R., & Ehlers, A. (2000). Dysfunctional meaning of posttraumatic intrusions in chronic PTSD. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38(6), 537–558.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Teasdale, J. D. (1983). Change in cognition during depression-psychopathological implications: Discussion paper. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 76(12), 1038–1044.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Teasdale, J. D., Moore, R. G., Hayhurst, H., Pope, M., Williams, S., & Segal, Z. V. (2002). Metacognitive awareness and prevention of relapse in depression: Empirical evidence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70(2), 275–287.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., Ridgeway, V. A., Soulsby, J. M., & Lau, M. A. (2000). Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68(4), 615–623.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(3), 247–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wahl, K., Huelle, J. O., Zurowski, B., & Kordon, A. (2013). Managing obsessive thoughts during brief exposure: An experimental study comparing mindfulness-based strategies and distraction in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 37(4), 752–761.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, E. (2008). Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychological Bulletin, 134(2), 163–206.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, E., Teasdale, J. D., & Williams, R. M. (2000). Decentering and distraction reduce overgeneral autobiographical memory in depression. Psychological Medicine, 30(4), 911–920.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, A., & Matthews, G. (1994). Attention and emotion: A clinical perspective. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Haruyuki Ishikawa.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethics Statement

This research was approved by the Ethics Review Committee on Research with Human Subjects of Waseda University.

Statement of Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all the participants included in the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ishikawa, H., Mieda, T., Oshio, A. et al. The Relationship Between Decentering and Adaptiveness of Response Styles in Reducing Depression. Mindfulness 9, 556–563 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-017-0797-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-017-0797-8

Keywords

Navigation