Advertisement

Measuring Affective Responses to Cuteness and Japanese kawaii as a Multidimensional Construct

  • Reina Takamatsu
Article
  • 142 Downloads

Abstract

The aim of this study was twofold: first to create a self-report scale that measures responsiveness to cuteness and kawaii in three key domains (baby animals, baby humans, and animate objects), and secondly to apply the measure to predicting parental attitudes toward corporal punishment. Currently, there are no self-report measures of this construct in theoretically consistent domains that encompass both cuteness and kawaii. In Study 1, a 15-item Cuteness Responsiveness (CR-15) scale was developed to assess responsiveness to infantile human and non-human kawaii creatures, which is defined as individual differences in the sensitivity to cuteness and readiness for a variety of caretaking behaviors. The CR-15 demonstrated evidence of good psychometric properties and fit into the three-factor structure. In Study 2, we recruited parents with a child/children under the age of 6 to test how responsiveness to cuteness affects parenting using the scale developed in Study 1. The results showed lower responsiveness to cuteness predicted parental approval for corporal punishment, and negative attitudes toward parenting mediated the link. Together, these studies demonstrate that the human baby subscale of the CR-15 can predict a tendency to experience positive emotions in response to infantile creatures and motivation for caretaking behaviors. Implications of cuteness/kawaii responses as a multidimensional construct and future directions are discussed.

Keywords

Cuteness Baby schema effect Scale development Parenting 

Notes

Acknowledgements

I would like to express my sincere gratitude to Dr. Hiroshi Nittono who provided helpful comments on the conceptualization of kawaii as a positive emotion.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Conflict of Interest

There is no potential conflict of interest pertaining to this submission to Current Psychology.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

References

  1. Alley, T. R. (1981). Head shape and the perception of cuteness. Developmental Psychology, 17, 650–654.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Alley, T. R. (1983). Age-related changes in body proportions, body size, and perceived cuteness. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 56, 615–622.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Aragon, O. R., Clark, M. S., Dyer, R. L., & Bargh, J. A. (2015). Dimorphous expressions of positive emotion: Displays of both care and aggression in response to cute stimuli. Psychological Science, 26, 259–273.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614561044.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Batson, C. D., Fultz, J., & Schoenrade, P. A. (1987). Distress and empathy: Two qualitatively distinct vicarious emotions with different motivational consequences. Journal of Personality, 55, 19–39.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Bennett, D. S., Sullivan, M. W., & Lewis, M. (2006). Relations of parental report and observation of parenting to maltreatment history. Child Maltreatment, 11, 63–75.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  6. Black, D. A., Heyman, R. E., & Smith Slep, A. M. (2001). Risk factors for child physical abuse. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 6, 121–188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Borgi, M., Cogliati-Dezza, I., Brelsford, V., Meints, K., & Cirulli, F. (2014). Baby schema in human and animal faces induces cuteness perception and gaze allocation in children. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Buckels, E. E., Beall, A. T., Hofer, M. K., Lin, E. Y., Zhou, Z., & Schaller, M. (2015). Individual differences in activation of the parental care motivational system: Assessment, prediction, and implications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108, 497–514.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Cohen, S., Kamarch, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24, 385–396.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Crouch, J. L., & Behl, L. E. (2001). Relationships among parental beliefs in corporal punishment, reported stress, and physical child abuse potential. Child Abuse & Neglect, 25, 413–419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Daly, M., & Wilson, M. I. (1985). Child abuse and other risks of not living with both parents. Ethology and Sociobiology, 6, 197–210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Damashek, A., Nelson, M. M., & Bonner, B. L. (2013). Fatal child maltreatment: Characteristics of deaths from physical abuse versus neglect. Child Abuse & Neglect, 37, 735–744.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Davis, M. H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS. Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10, 85.Google Scholar
  14. Deater-Deckard, K., Smith, J., Ivy, L., & Petril, S. A. (2005). Differential perceptions of and feelings about sibling children: Implications for research on parenting stress. Infant and Child Development, 14, 211–225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Egeland, B., Breitenbucher, M., & Rosenberg, D. (1980). Prospective study of the significance of life stress in the etiology of child abuse. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 48, 195–205.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Epley, N., Waytz, A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2007). On seeing human: A three-factor theory of anthropomorphism. Psychological Review, 114, 864–886.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Fleming, A., Corter, C., Stallings, J., & Steiner, M. (2002). Testosterone and prolactin associated with emotional responses to infant cries in new fathers. Hormones and Behavior, 42, 399–413.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Frodi, A. M., & Lamb, M. E. (1980). Child abusers’ responses to infant smiles and cries. Child Development, 51, 238–241.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Fujiwara, T., Yamaoka, Y., & Kawachi, I. (2016). Neighborhood social capital and infant physical abuse: A population-based study in Japan. International Journal of Mental Health Systems, 10, 13–19.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  20. Glocker, M. L., Langleben, D. D., Ruparel, K., Loughead, J. W., Gur, R. C., & Sachser, N. (2009). Baby schema in infant induces cuteness perception and motivation for caretaking in adults. Ethology, 115, 257–263.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  21. Graham, J., Haidt, J., Koleva, S., Motyl, M., Iyer, R., Wojcik, S. P., & Ditto, P. H. (2013). Moral foundations theory: The pragmatic validity of moral pluralism. In P. Devine & A. Plant (Eds.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 55–130). London: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  22. Graham, J., Nosek, B. A., Haidt, J., Iyer, R., Koleva, S., & Ditto, P. H. (2011). Mapping the moral domain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 366–385.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  23. Granziano, A. M. (1994). Why we should study subabusive violence against children. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 9, 412–419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Haidt, J. (2012). The righteous mind: Why good people are divided by politics and religion. New York: Vintage Books.Google Scholar
  25. Hildebrandt, K. A., & Fitzgerald, H. E. (1978). Adults’ responses to infants varying in perceived cuteness. Behavioural Processes, 3, 159–172.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Hilson, J. M. C., & Kuiper, N. A. (1994). A stress and coping model of child maltreatment. Clinical Psychology Review, 14, 261–285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Honda, S., Ishimaru, A., Utsunomiya, S., Yamane, T., Oda, M., Sakamoto, K., Ohe, Y., Kobayashi, H., Arima, T., Kidera, A., & Kosugi, K. (2016). Considering the relation between the Haidt’s moral foundation theory and Japanese morality through developing a new scale. Bulletin of the Faculty of Education, Yamaguchi University, 66, 95–106 Retrieved from http://www.lib.yamaguchi-u.ac.jp/yunoca/handle/C010066000311.Google Scholar
  28. Kadushin, A., & Martin, J. A. (1981). Interview study of abuse-event interaction. In A. Kadushin (Ed.), Child abuse: An interactional event (pp. 141–224). New York, NY: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
  29. Kanai, Y., & Nittono, H. (2015). Predictive modeling of the feelings of kawaii by empathy and affiliation motives. (In Japanese with English abstract). The Japanese Journal of Personality, 23, 131–141.  https://doi.org/10.2132/personality.23.131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Kringelbach, M. L., Stark, E. A., Alexander, C., Bornstein, M. H., & Stein, A. (2016). On cuteness: Unlocking the parental brain and beyond. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20, 545–558.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.05.0035.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  31. Lau, A. S., Valeri, S. M., McCarty, C. A., & Weisz, J. R. (2006). Abusive parents’ reports of child behavior problems: Relationship to observed parent-child interactions. Child Abuse & Neglect, 30, 639–655.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Lehmann, V., Huisin’t Veld, E. M., & Vingerhoets, A. J. (2013). The human and animal baby schema effect: Correlates of individual differences. Behavioural Processes, 94, 99–108.Google Scholar
  33. Little, A. C. (2012). Manipulation of infant-like traits affects perceived cuteness of infant, adult and cat faces. Ethology, 118, 775–782.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2012.02068.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Lobmaier, J. S., Probst, F., Perrett, D. I., & Heinrichs, M. (2015). Menstrual cycle phase affects discrimination of infant cuteness. Hormones and Behavior, 70, 1–6.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Mauss, I. B., & Robinson, M. D. (2009). Measures of emotion: A review. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 209–237.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  36. May-Chahal, C., & Cawson, P. (2005). Measuring child maltreatment in the United Kingdom: A study of the prevalence of child abuse and neglect. Child Abuse & Neglect, 29, 969–984.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Miesler, L., Leder, H., & Herrmann, A. (2011). Isn’t it cute: An evolutionary perspective of baby-schema effects in visual product designs. International Journal of Design, 5, 17–30.Google Scholar
  38. Milner, J. S., & Chilamkurti, C. (1991). Physical child abuse perpetrator characteristics: A review of the literature. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 6, 345–366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Milner, J. S., Rabenhorst, M. M., McCanne, T. R., Crouch, J. L., Skowronski, J. J., Fleming, M. T., Hiraoka, R., & Risser, H. J. (2011). Event-related potentials: Search for positive and negative child-related schemata in individuals at low ad high risk for child physical abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 35, 249–266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Nisbett, R. W., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Nittono, H. (2016). The two-layer model of “kawaii”: A behavioural science framework for understanding kawaii and cuteness. East Asian Journal of Popular Culture Retrieved from http://cplnet.jp/swfu/d/Nittono2016-Kawaii-review.pdf.
  42. Nittono, H., Fukushima, M., Yano, A., & Moriya, H. (2012). The power of kawaii: Viewing cute images promotes a careful behavior and narrows attentional focus. PLoS One, 7, e46362.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  43. Nittono, H., & Ihara, N. (2017). Psychophysiological responses to kawaii pictures with or without baby schema. SAGE Open, 7, 1–11.  https://doi.org/10.1177/2158244017709321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Ohinata, M. (1988). Bosei no kenkyu (maternity research). Kawashima Shoten: Tokyo.Google Scholar
  45. Paulhus, D. L., & Vazire, S. (2007). The self-report method. In R. W. Robbins, R. C. Fraley, & R. F. Krueger (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in personality psychology (pp. 224–239). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  46. Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 717–731.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Proverbio, A. M., de Garbriele, V., Manfredi, M., & Adorni, R. (2011). No race effect (ORE) in the automatic orienting toward baby faces: When ethnic group does not mater. Psychology, 2, 931–935.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Reizer, A., & Mikulincer, M. (2007). Assessing individual differences in working models of caregiving: The construction and validation of the mental representation of caregiving scale. Journal of Individual Differences, 28, 227–239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Robson, K. S., & Moss, H. A. (1970). Patterns and determinants of maternal attachment. Journal of Pediatrics, 77, 976–985.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Rodriguez, C. M., & Richardson, M. J. (2007). Stress and anger as contextual factors and preexisting cognitive schemas: Predicting parental child maltreatment risk. Child Maltreatment, 12, 325–337.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Rodriguez, C. M., & Tucker, M. C. (2015). Predicting maternal physical child abuse risk beyond distress and social support: Additive role of cognitive processes. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 24, 1780–1790.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Sakurai, S. (1988). The relationship between empathy and helping behavior in college students. Bulletin of Nara University of Education, 37, 1(Cultural and Social Science), 149–153.Google Scholar
  53. Sherman, G. D., & Haidt, J. (2011). Cuteness and disgust: The humanizing and dehumanizing effects of emotion. Emotion Review, 3, 1–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Sherman, G. D., Haidt, J., & Coan, J. A. (2009). Viewing cute images increases behavioral carefulness. Emotion, 9, 282–286.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Sherman, G. D., Haidt, J., Iyer, R., & Coan, J. A. (2013). Individual differences in the physical embodiment of care: Prosocially oriented women respond to cuteness by becoming more physically careful. Emotion, 13, 151–158.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Sprengelmeyer, R., Perrett, D., Fagan, E. C., Cornwell, R. E., Lobmaier, J. S., Sprengelmeyer, A., Aasheim, H. B. M., Black, I. M., Cameron, L. M., Crow, S., Milne, N., Rhodes, E. C., & Young, A. W. (2009). The cutest little baby face: A hormonal link to sensitivity to cuteness in infant faces. Psychological Science, 20, 149–154.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Stith, S. M., Liu, T., Davies, C., Boykin, E. L., Alder, M. C., Harris, J. M., Som, A., McPherson, M., & Dees, J. E. M. E. G. (2009). Risk factors in child maltreatment: A meta-analytic review of the literature. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 14, 13-29.Google Scholar
  58. Stoltenborgh, M., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Alink, L. R. A., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2015). The prevalence of child maltreatment across the globe: Review of a series of meta-analyses. Child Abuse Review, 24, 37–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Straus, M. A. (1994). Should the use of corporal punishment by parents be considered child abuse? Yes. In M. A. Mason & E. Gambrill (Eds.), Debating children’s lives: Current controversies on children and adolescents (pp. 197–203). Thousand Oaks: Sage.Google Scholar
  60. Sumi, K. (2006). Reliability and validity of the Japanese version of the perceived stress scale. The Japanese Journal of Health Psychology, 19, 44–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Swain, J. E., Konrath, S., & Ho, S. S. (2012). Parenting and beyond: Common neurocircuits underlying parental and altruistic caregiving. Parenting, science, and practice, 12, 115–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. The Japanese Ministry of Health, Labour, and Welfare. (2012). Child abuse facts and implication for future policies. Retrieved from: http://www.mhlw.go.jp/seisakunitsuite/bunya/kodomo/kodomo_kosodate/dv/about.html
  63. The Japanese Ministry of Health, Labour, and Welfare. (2015). Guidelines for the prevention of child abuse. Retrieved from: http://www.mhlw.go.jp/bunya/kodomo/dv12/00.html
  64. Trickett, P. K., & Kuczynski, L. (1986). Children’s misbehaviors and parental discipline strategies in abusive and nonabusive families. Developmental Psychology, 22, 115–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Tucker, M. C., & Rodriguez, C. M. (2014). Family dysfunction and social isolation as moderators between stress and child physical abuse risk. Journal of Family Violence, 29, 175–186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Administration on Children, Youth, and Families. (2007). Child maltreatment 2005. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.Google Scholar
  67. US Department of Health and Human Services (2010). Administration on Children, Youth and Families: Child Maltreatment 2008. Retrieved from: http://www.acf.hhs.gov.
  68. WHO - World Health Organization and International Society for Prevention of Child Abuse and Neglect. (2006). Preventing child maltreatment: A guide to taking action and generating evidence. Geneva: WHO Press.Google Scholar
  69. Yano, C. R. (2013). Pink Globalization: Hello Kitty and Its Trek Across the Pacific. Durham. Duke University Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Psychology and Human Developmental SciencesNagoya UniversityNagoyaJapan

Personalised recommendations