Skip to main content
Log in

Combining implicit and explicit techniques to reveal social desirability bias in electricity conservation self-reports

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Energy Efficiency Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

While questionnaires are still the most common way to survey consumers’ behaviors, it is known that respondents’ answers can be affected by the social desirability attributed to the behavior under investigation. To check whether a social desirability bias also affects electricity consumption self-reports, a study was carried out adopting an explicit (questionnaire) and implicit measurement technique (the autobiographical Implicit Association Test). Three behaviors were probed in this way, with a sample of 180 participants (60 for each behavior). The analysis of the congruence between explicit and implicit answers confirms that desirability bias is at stake in self-reported measures of electricity conservation; it also shows that different behaviors—in this same domain—can be subject to this bias to a different extent and that a considerable amount of participants need to be considered as ambivalent. The methodological and conceptual implications of these findings and of the method are discussed with respect to pro-environmental studies and interventions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. A demo page with several different IATs is offered at this URL: https://implicit.harvard.edu/implicit/takeatest.html.

  2. The D index includes a penalty for incorrect trials and expresses the IAT effect (the difference in performance between the two double-categorization blocks) in terms of the standard deviation of the latency measures. It is calculated by subtracting corrected mean RTs in the fifth block from mean RTs in the third block and then dividing the result by the inclusive standard deviation of the two blocks.

References

  • Agosta S., Sartori G. (2013) The autobiographical IAT: A review. Frontiers of Psychology, 4, article n. 00519, retrieved from http://www.frontiersin.org/Journal/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00519/full.

  • Antil, J. H. (1979). Construction and validation of a scale to measure socially responsible consumption behavior. In K. E. Hennion II & T. C. Kinnear (Eds.), The conserver society. Chicago: Proceeding Series, American Marketing Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bain, P. G., Hornsey, M. J., Bongiorno, R., & Jeffries, C. (2012). Promoting pro-environmental action in climate change deniers. Nature Climate Change, 2(8), 600–603.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bamberg, S. (2003). How does environmental concern influence specific environmentally related behaviors? A new answer to an old question. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 23, 21–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chung, J., & Monroe, G. S. (2003). Exploring social desirability bias. Journal of Business Ethics, 44(4), 291–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral science (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corral-Verdugo, V. (1997). Dual ‘realities’ of conservation behavior: Self-reports vs observations of re-use and recycling behavior. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 17(2), 135–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crossley, N. (2013). Habit and habitus. Body and Society, 19(2&3), 136–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Darby, S. (2006). The effectiveness of feedback on energy consumption. A review for DEFRA of the literature on metering, billing, and direct displays. Last accessed on November 2011 from: http://www.eci.ox.ac.uk/research/energy/downloads/smart-metering-report.pdf.

  • Dasgupta, N., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). The First Ontological Challenge to the IAT: Attitude or Mere Familiarity? Psychological Inquiry, 14(3–4), 238–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Groot, J. I. M., & Steg, L. (2010). Relationships between value orientations, self-determined motivational types and pro-environmental behavioural intentions. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 30(4), 368–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Young, R. (1986). Some psychological aspects of recycling. The structure of conservation satisfaction. Environment and Behavior, 18, 435–449.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Young, R. (1991). Some psychological aspects of living lightly: Desired lifestyle patterns and conservation behavior. Journal of Environmental Systems, 20(3), 215–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De, H., De Houwer, J., Teige-Mocigemba, S., Spruyt, A., & Moors. (2009). Implicit measures: A normative analysis and review. Psychological Bulletin, 135(3), 347–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donders, F. C. (1969). On the speed of mental processes. Acta Psychologica, 30, 413-431. (Original paper published in 1868).

  • Ebreo, A. & Vining, J. (1994). Conservation-wise consumers: Recycling and household shopping as ecological behavior. Journal of Environmental Systems, 23, 109-131. *

  • Fazio, R. H. (1990). Multiple processes by which attitudes guide behavior: The MODE model as an integrative framework. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 75–109). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 297–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fielder, K., Messner, C., & Bluemke, M. (2006). Unresolved problems with the “I”, the “A”, and the “T”: A logical and psychometric critique of the Implicit Association Test (IAT). European Review of Social Psychology, 17, 74–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, C. (2008). Feedback on household electricity consumption: A tool for saving energy? Energy Efficiency, 1(1), 79–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, R. J. (1993). Social desirability bias and the validity of indirect questioning. The Journal of Consumer Research, 20(2), 303–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Froehlich J., Findlater L., & Landay J. (2010). The design of eco-feedback technology. In Proceedings of the 28th international conference on Human factors in computing systems (CHI '10), (pp. 1999–2008). New York: ACM.

  • Goldenhar, L. M., & Connell, C. M. (1993). Understanding and predicting recycling behavior: An application of the theory of reasoned action. Journal of Environmental Systems, 22, 91–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1464–1480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 197–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirst, E., & Goeltz, R. (1985). Accuracy of self-reports: Energy conservation surveys. The Social Science Journal, 22(1), 19–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kantola, S. J., Syme, G. J., & Campbell, N. A. (1984). Cognitive dissonance and energy conservation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 69(3), 416–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, K. (2005). The effects of nonnormal distributions on confidence intervals around the standardized mean difference: bootstrap and parametric confidence intervals. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 65(1), 51–69.

    Article  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  • Kollmuss, A., & Agyeman, J. (2002). Mind the gap: why do people act environmentally and what are the barriers to pro-environmental behavior? Environmental Education Research, 8(3), 239–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, S. J. (1981). Interpreting consumer mythology: A structural approach to consumer behavior. Journal of Marketing, 45, 49–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Linn, N., Vining, J., & Feeley, P. (1994). Toward a sustainable society: Waste minimization through environmentally conscious consuming. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24(17), 1550–1572.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McConahay, J. B., Hardee, B. B., & Batts, V. (1981). Has racism declined in America? It depends on who is asking and what is asked. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 25(4), 563–579.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGuire, W. J. (1986). The vicissitudes of attitudes and similar representational constructs in twentieth century psychology. European Journal of Social Psychology, 16, 89–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, P., Laczniac, G. R., & Robinson, R. K. (1979). An attitudinal and behavioral index of energy conservation. In K. E. Henion II & T. C. Kinnear (Eds.), The Conserver Society. Chicago: Proceeding Series, American Marketing Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nocera, F., & Ferlazzo, F. (2000). Resampling approach to statistical inference: Bootstrapping from event-related potentials data. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 32(1), 111–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parker, R. I. (2006). Increased reliability for single-case research results: is the bootstrap the answer? Behavior Therapy, 37(4), 326–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peltier, B. D., & Walsh, J. A. (1990). An investigation of response bias in the Chapman Scales. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 50(4), 803–815.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pierce, J., Strengers, Y., Sengers, P., and Bødker, S. 2013. Introduction to the special issue on practice-oriented approaches to sustainable HCI. ACM Trans. Comput.-Hum. Interact. 20, 4, Article 20 (September 2013), 8 pages

  • Robinette, R. L. (1991). The relationship between the Marlowe-Crowne Form C and the validity scales of the MMPI. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 47(3), 396–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sartori, G., Agosta, S., Zogmaister, C., Ferrara, S. D., & Castiello, U. (2008). How to accurately detect autobiographical events. Psychological Science, 19(8), 772–780.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, J., & Simon, R. (1975). The effect of money incentives on family size: A hypothetical-question study. Public Opinion Quarterly, 38, 585–595.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suchman, L., & Jordan, B. (1992). Validity and the collaborative construction of meaning in face-to-face surveys. In J. Tanur (Ed.), Questions about Questions: Inquiries into the Cognitive Bases of Surveys (pp. 241–267). New York, NY: Russell Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tracy, A. P., & Oskamp, S. (1984). Relationship among ecologically responsible behaviors. Journal of Environmental Systems, 13(2), 115–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5(2), 207–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uhlmann, E. L., Poehlman, T. A., & Nosek, B. A. (2012). Automatic associations: Personal attitudes or cultural knowledge? In J. Hanson (Ed.), Ideology, Psychology, and Law (pp. 228–260). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Van Raaij, W. F., & Verhallen, T. M. (1983). A behavioral model of residential energy use. Journal of Economic Psychology, 3(1), 39–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitford, J. (2002). Pragmatism and the untenable dualism of means and ends: why rational choice theory does not deserve paradigmatic privilege. Theory and Society, 31, 325–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitley, E. E., & Kite, M. E. (2010). The psychology of prejudice and discrimination. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitmarsh, L., & O’Neill, S. (2010). Green identity, green living? The role of pro-environmental self-identity in determining consistency across diverse pro-environmental behaviours. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 30(3), 305–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woods, J. (2008). What people do when they say they are conserving electricity. Energy Policy, 36(6), 1945–1956.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zerbe, W. J., & Paulhus, D. L. (1987). Socially desirable responding in organizational behavior: A reconception. The Academy of Management Review, 12(2), 250–264.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work has been co-funded by the European Union (EU FP7/ICT-2007.6.3, project no. 224557 'BeAware', http://energyawareness.eu). The authors would also like to thank the “Museo Antoniano” and two public libraries, “Brenta-Venezia” and “Emeroteca,” in Padua, for hosting the data collection, and their visitors for participating in the study. They also would like to thank Giulia Bosetti and Pietro Zappaterra for helping in the data collection and Francesco Martino for advising on an earlier version of the work.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Luciano Gamberini.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gamberini, L., Spagnolli, A., Corradi, N. et al. Combining implicit and explicit techniques to reveal social desirability bias in electricity conservation self-reports. Energy Efficiency 7, 923–935 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12053-014-9266-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12053-014-9266-6

Keywords

Navigation