Synthese

, Volume 195, Issue 5, pp 1873–1889 | Cite as

A neuropsychological challenge to the sentimentalism/rationalism distinction

S.I.: Neuroscience and Its Philosophy

Abstract

Critical reflection on the available neuropsychological evidence suggests that the roles of emotion and reason in moral judgment may not be distinct. This casts significant doubt on our current understanding of moral judgment, and therefore also on all philosophical theories based on that understanding. Most notably, it raises doubts about both sentimentalism and rationalism, which historically have often been treated as exclusive and exhaustive theories regarding the nature of moral concepts. As an alternative, I endorse pluralism with regard to the emotional and rational nature of moral concepts.

Keywords

Sentimentalism Rationalism Pluralism Emotion Reason Cognition Neuroethics Moral psychology Dual-process model Joshua Greene Moral judgment task Dilemmas Trolley problems 

Notes

Acknowledgements

I am grateful to Joshua Greene and to Michael Davis, Thomas Fisher, Elisabeth Hildt, Warren Schmaus, Aaron Spink, and two anonymous referees for their comments on earlier drafts of this paper. My research was funded by a grant from the Swiss Cogito Foundation, to which I am also grateful.

References

  1. Berker, S. (2009). The normative insignificance of neuroscience. Philosophy & Public Affairs, 37(4), 293–329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Blanke, O., Ortigue, S., Landis, T., & Seeck, M. (2002). Stimulating illusory own-body perceptions. Nature, 419(6904), 269–270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bromm, B. (2004). The involvement of the posterior cingulate gyrus in phasic pain processing of humans. Neuroscience Letters, 361(1–3), 245–249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain’s default network: Anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 1–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Carter, R. M., O’Doherty, J. P., Seymour, B., Koch, C., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Contingency awareness in human aversive conditioning involves the middle frontal gyrus. NeuroImage, 29(3), 1007–1012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Cera, N., Di Pierro, E. D., Sepede, G., Gambi, F., Perrucci, M. G., Merla, A., et al. (2012). The role of left superior parietal lobe in male sexual behavior: Dynamics of distinct components revealed by FMRI. The Journal of Sexual Medicine, 9(6), 1602–1612.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Christensen, J. F., & Gomila, A. (2012). Moral dilemmas in cognitive neuroscience of moral decision-making: A principled review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(4), 1249–1264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Ciaramelli, E., Muccioli, M., Ladavas, E., & di Pellegrino, G. (2007). Selective deficit in personal moral judgment following damage to ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2(2), 84–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Dolcos, F., Iordan, A. D., & Dolcos, S. (2011). Neural correlates of emotion-cognition interactions: A review of evidence from brain imaging investigations. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23(6), 669–694.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Farrer, C., Frey, S. H., Van Horn, J. D., Tunik, E., Turk, D., Inati, S., et al. (2008). The angular gyrus computes action awareness representations. Cerebral Cortex, 18(2), 254–261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Foot, P. (1967). The problem of abortion and the doctrine of the double effect. Oxford Review, 5, 5–15.Google Scholar
  12. Gallagher, H. L., Happé, F., Brunswick, N., Fletcher, P. C., Frith, U., & Frith, C. D. (2000). Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: An fMRI study of “theory of mind” in verbal and nonverbal tasks. Neuropsychologia, 38(1), 11–21.Google Scholar
  13. Garrigan, B., Adlam, A. L. R., & Langdon, P. E. (2016). The neural correlates of moral decision-making: A systematic review and meta-analysis of moral evaluations and response decision judgements. Brain and Cognition, 108, 88–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Göbel, S., Walsh, V., & Rushworth, M. F. (2001). The mental number line and the human angular gyrus. NeuroImage, 14(6), 1278–1289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Goldin, P. R., McRae, K., Ramel, W., & Gross, J. J. (2008). The neural bases of emotion regulation: Reappraisal and suppression of negative emotion. Biological Psychiatry, 63(6), 577–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Grabner, R. H., Ansari, D., Koschutnig, K., Reishofer, G., Ebner, F., & Neuper, C. (2009a). To retrieve or to calculate? Left angular gyrus mediates the retrieval of arithmetic facts during problem solving. Neuropsychologia, 47(2), 604–608.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Grabner, R. H., Ischebeck, A., Reishofer, G., Koschutnig, K., Delazer, M., Ebner, F., et al. (2009b). Fact learning in complex arithmetic and figural-spatial tasks: The role of the angular gyrus and its relation to mathematical competence. Human Brain Mapping, 30(9), 2936–2952.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Gray, K., & Schein, C. (2012). Two minds vs. two philosophies: Mind perception defines morality and dissolves the debate between deontology and utilitarianism. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 3(3), 405–423.Google Scholar
  19. Greene, J. D. (2007). Why are VMPFC patients more utilitarian? A dual-process theory of moral judgment explains. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(8), 322–323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Greene, J. D. (2014a). Beyond point-and-shoot morality: Why cognitive (neuro)science matters for ethics. Ethics, 124(4), 695–726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Greene, J. D. (2014b). The cognitive neuroscience of moral judgment and decision making. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The Cognitive Neurosciences V (pp. 1013–1024). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  22. Greene, J. D., Nystrom, L. E., Engell, A. D., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). The neural bases of cognitive conflict and control in moral judgment. Neuron, 44(2), 389–400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2001a). An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science (New York, N.Y.), 293, 2105–2108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2001b). An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. [Supplementary material]. Science (New York, N.Y.), 293(5537), 2105–8. http://science.sciencemag.org/content/293/5537/2105/tab-figures-data.
  25. Grèzes, J., Pichon, S., & de Gelder, B. (2007). Perceiving fear in dynamic body expressions. NeuroImage, 35(2), 959–967.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Helion, C., & Pizarro, D. A. (2015). Beyond dual-processes: the interplay of reason and emotion in moral judgment. In J. Clausen & N. Levy (Eds.), Handbook of Neuroethics (pp. 109–125). SpringerGoogle Scholar
  27. Hintikka, J. (1998). What is abduction? The fundamental problem of contemporary epistemology. Transactions of the Charles S Peirce Society, 34(3), 503–533.Google Scholar
  28. Hirono, N., Mori, E., Ishii, K., Ikejiri, Y., Imamura, T., Shimomura, T., et al. (1998). Hypofunction in the posterior cingulate gyrus correlates with disorientation for time and place in Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry, 64, 552–554.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Horwitz, B., Rumsey, J. M., & Donohue, B. C. (1998). Functional connectivity of the angular gyrus in normal reading and dyslexia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95(15), 8939–8944.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hume, D. (2013/1751). An enquiry concerning the principles of morals. New York: Start Publishing LLC.Google Scholar
  31. Iacoboni, M., Molnar-Szakacs, I., Gallese, V., Buccino, G., Mazziotta, J. C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Grasping the intentions of others with one’s own mirror neuron system. Plos Biology, 3, e79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Jacoby, L. L. (1991). A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30(5), 513–541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Kamm, F. M. (2009). Neuroscience and moral reasoning: A note on recent research. Philosophy & Public Affairs, 37(4), 330–345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Kant, I. (1964/1785). Groundwork of the metaphysic of morals (HJ Paton, Trans.). NY: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  35. Katayama, K., Takahashi, N., Ogawara, K., & Hattori, T. (1999). Pure topographical disorientation due to right posterior cingulate lesion. Cortex, 35(2), 279–282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Klein, C. (2011). The dual track theory of moral decision-making: A critique of the neuroimaging evidence. Neuroethics, 4(2), 143–162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Koenigs, M., Holliday, J., Solomon, J., & Grafman, J. (2010). Left dorsomedial frontal brain damage is associated with insomnia. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(47), 16041–16043.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Koenigs, M., Young, L., Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Cushman, F., Hauser, M., et al. (2007). Damage to the prefrontal cortex increases utilitarian moral judgements. Nature, 446(7138), 908–911.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Kosslyn, S., Shin, L., & Thompson, W. (1996). Neural effects of visualizing and perceiving aversive stimuli: A PET investigation. Neuroreport, 7, 1569–1576.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Machery, E. (2013). In defense of reverse inference. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 65(2), 251–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Maddock, R. J. (1999). The retrosplenial cortex and emotion: new insights from functional neuroimaging of the human brain. Trends in Neurosciences, 22(7), 310–316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Maddock, R. J., & Buonocore, M. H. (1997). Activation of left posterior cingulate gyrus by the auditory presentation ofthreat-related words: An fMRI study. Psychiatry Research - Neuroimaging, 75(1), 1–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Miller, M. B., Sinnott-Armstrong, W., Young, L., King, D., Paggi, A., Fabri, M., et al. (2010). Abnormal moral reasoning in complete and partial callosotomy patients. Neuropsychologia, 48(7), 2215–2220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Moll, J., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Bramati, I. E., & Grafman, J. (2002). Functional networks in emotional moral and nonmoral social judgments. NeuroImage, 16(3), 696–703.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Monks, P. J., Thompson, J. M., Bullmore, E. T., Suckling, J., Brammer, M. J., Williams, S. C. R., et al. (2004). A functional MRI study of working memory task in euthymic bipolar disorder: Evidence for task-specific dysfunction. Bipolar Disorders, 6(6), 550–564.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Muller, J., Sommer, M., Wagner, V., Wagner, V., Lange, K., Lange, K., et al. (2003). Abnormalities in emotion processing within cortical and subcortical regions in criminal psychopaths: Evidence from a functional magnetic resonance imaging study using pictures with emotional content. Biological Psychiatry, 54(2), 152–162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Mundy, P. (2003). Annotation: The neural basis of social impairments in autism: The role of the dorsal medial-frontal cortex and anterior cingulate system. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 44(6), 793–809.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Penner-Wilger, M., & Anderson, M. L. (2013). The relation between finger gnosis and mathematical ability: Why redeployment of neural circuits best explains the finding. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Poldrack, R. A. (2006). Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(2), 59–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Poldrack, R. A. (2011). Inferring mental states from neuroimaging data: From reverse inference to large-scale decoding. Neuron, 72(5), 692–697.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Price, G. R., & Ansari, D. (2011). Symbol processing in the left angular gyrus: Evidence from passive perception of digits. NeuroImage, 57(3), 1205–1211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Pugh, K. R., Mencl, W. E., Shaywitz, B. A., Shaywitz, S. E., Fulbright, R. K., Constable, R. T., et al. (2000). The angular gyrus in developmental dyslexia: Task-specific differences in functional connectivity within posterior cortex. Psychological Science, 11(1), 51–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Reiman, E. M., Lane, R. D., Ahern, G. L., Schwartz, G. E., Davidson, R. J., Friston, K. J., et al. (1997). Neuroanatomical correlates of externally and internally generated human emotion. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154(7), 918–925.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Rusconi, E., Walsh, V., & Butterworth, B. (2005). Dexterity with numbers: rTMS over left angular gyrus disrupts finger gnosis and number processing. Neuropsychologia, 43(11), 1609–1624.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Schienle, A., Schäfer, A., Walter, B., Stark, R., & Vaitl, D. (2005a). Brain activation of spider phobics towards disorder-relevant, generally disgust- and fear-inducing pictures. Neuroscience Letters, 388(1), 1–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Schienle, A., Schäfer, A., Stark, R., Walter, B., & Vaitl, D. (2005b). Gender differences in the processing of disgust- and fear-inducing pictures: An fMRI study. Neuroreport, 16(3), 277–280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Seghier, M. (2012). The angular gyrus: Multiple functions and multiple subdivisions. The Neuroscientist, 19(1), 43–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Shenhav, A., & Greene, J. D. (2014). Integrative moral judgment: Dissociating the roles of the amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(13), 4741–4749.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Singer, P. (1972). Famine, affluence and morality. Philosophy and Public Affairs, 1(3), 229–243.Google Scholar
  60. Singer, P. (2005). Ethics and intuitions. The Journal of Ethics, 9(3–4), 331–352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Small, D. M., Gitelman, D. R., Gregory, M. D., Nobre, A. C., Parrish, T. B., & Mesulam, M. M. (2003). The posterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex mediate the anticipatory allocation of spatial attention. NeuroImage, 18(3), 633–641.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Talati, A., & Hirsch, J. (2005). Functional specialization within the medial frontal gyrus for perceptual go/no-go decisions based on “what,” “when,” and “where” related information: an fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(7), 981–993.Google Scholar
  63. Van Orden, G. C., Pennington, B. F., & Stone, G. O. (2001). What do double dissociations prove? Cognitive Science, 25(1), 111–172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Vincent, J. L., Kahn, I., Snyder, A. Z., Raichle, M. E., & Buckner, R. L. (2008). Evidence for a frontoparietal control system revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. Journal of Neurophysioloy, 100, 3328–3342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Vogt, B. A. (2005). Pain and emotion interactions in subregions of the cingulate gyrus. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(7), 533–544.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Young, L., Camprodon, J. A., Hauser, M., Pascual-Leone, A., & Saxe, R. (2010). Disruption of the right temporoparietal junction with transcranial magnetic stimulation reduces the role of beliefs in moral judgments. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(15), 6753–6758.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Young, L., & Saxe, R. (2008). The neural basis of belief encoding and integration in moral judgment. NeuroImage, 40(4), 1912–1920.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Center for the Study of Ethics in the ProfessionsIllinois Institute of TechnologyChicagoUSA

Personalised recommendations