Advertisement

Research in Science Education

, Volume 46, Issue 1, pp 21–42 | Cite as

The Contribution of the Human Body in Young Children’s Explanations About Shadow Formation

  • Evagelia Herakleioti
  • Panagiotis Pantidos
Article

Abstract

This paper begins with the view that the generation of meaning is a multimodal process. Props, drawings, graphs, gestures, as well as speech and written text are all mediators through which students construct new knowledge. Each semiotic context makes a unique contribution to the conceptualization of scientific entities. The human body, in particular, can function as a factor in both representation and explanation, serving as a link between verbal discourse and setting. Considering this perspective, a body-based activity was designed for kindergarten children, involving the concept of a shadow. The 3-D arrangement of the light from the light source, the human body (the obstacle), and the resulting shadow plays a central role. Using their own bodies as obstacles to the light, the children were able to explore the direction of the light and to change the relative positions of the light source and the obstacle. They formed hypotheses and were able to test them by moving on the stage. This body-centered activity explicitly incorporates the rectilinear movement of light into the process of shadow formation, while also providing learning through direct experience. Positive effects on learning were achieved for the group of children who participated in the activity, while the video analysis showed that many of the children were able to use their bodies to transfer to a different setting the embodied knowledge they acquired. This, according to researchers in the field of science education, is a powerful indication of conceptual change.

Keywords

Shadow formation Human body Embodied knowledge Transfer of knowledge 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the artist Pantelis Constantinou for the drawings.

References

  1. Barab, S., & Dede, C. (2007). Games and immersive participatory simulations for science education: an emerging type of curricula. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 16(1), 1–3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Barrow, L. (2007). Bringing light onto shadows. Science and Children, 44(9), 43–45.Google Scholar
  3. Chang, N. (2005). Children’s drawings: science inquiry and beyond. Contemporary Issues in Early Childhood, 6(1), 1,104–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Chen, S.-M. (2009). Shadows: young Taiwanese children’s views and understanding. International Journal of Science Education, 31(1), 59–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Christidou, V., Kazela, K., Kakana, D., & Valakosta, M. (2009). Teaching magnetic attraction to preschool children: a comparison of different approaches. International Journal of Learning, 16(2), 115–128.Google Scholar
  6. de Ruiter, J. P. (2007). Postcards from the mind: the relationship between speech, imagistic gesture, and thought. Gesture, 7(1), 21–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Eraut, M. (2009). Transfer of knowledge between education and workplace settings. Knowledge, Values and Educational Policy: A Critical Perspective, 65.Google Scholar
  8. Fleer, M. (1996). Early learning about light: mapping preschool children’s thinking about light before, during and after involvement in a two week teaching program. International Journal of Science Education, 18(7), 819–836.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Georghiades, P. (2000). Beyond conceptual change learning in science education: focusing on transfer, durability and metacognition. Educational Research, 42(2), 119–139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Georgoutsou, M., Panagiotaki, A., & Koliopoulos, D. (2010). Application of an educational programme on the concept of the shadow through the use of simulations on the website of the museum Cité des Sciences et de l’ Industrie. In A. Tzimogiannis (ed.), Proceedings of the 7th panhellenic conference on ICT in education], vol. ΙΙ (pp. 727–735). University of the Peloponnese, Corinth, 23–26 September 2010 (in Greek).Google Scholar
  11. Givry, D., & Pantidos, P. (2012). Toward a multimodal approach of science teaching. Skhole, 17, 123–129.Google Scholar
  12. Givry, D., & Roth, W.-M. (2006). Toward a new conception of conceptions: interplay of talk, gestures, and structures in the setting. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 43, 1086–1109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Grammatikopoulos, V., Gregoriadis, A., & Zachopoulou, E. (2012). Acknowledging the role of motor domain in creativity in early childhood education. Contemporary perspectives on research in creativity in early childhood education, 159–176.Google Scholar
  14. Hadzigeorgiou, Y. (2003). Sound and light–water and air: a beginning in the natural sciences. Athens: Grigoris Press (in Greek).Google Scholar
  15. Hadzigeorgiou, Y., Anastasiou, L., Konsolas, M., & Prevezanou, B. (2009). A study of the effect of preschool children’s participation in sensorimotor activities on their understanding of the mechanical equilibrium of a balance beam. Research in Science Education, 39(1), 39–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Huffaker, D. A., & Calvert, S. L. (2003). The new science of learning: active learning, metacognition, and transfer of knowledge in e-learning applications. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 29(3), 325–334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Hwang, S., & Roth, W. M. (2011). The (embodied) performance of physics concepts in lectures. Research in Science Education, 41(4), 461–477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Jaipal, K. (2010). Meaning making through multiple modalities in a biology classroom: a multimodal semiotics discourse analysis. Science Education, 94(1), 48–72.Google Scholar
  19. Kamii, C., & Kato, Y. (Eds.). (2007). Piaget’s constructivism and early childhood education: I. physical knowledge activities. Okayama City: Daigaku Kyoiku.Google Scholar
  20. Kress, G. (1997). Before writing: rethinking the paths to literacy. London and New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  21. Kress, G., Jewitt, C., Ogborn, J., & Tsatsarelis, C. (2001). Multimodal teaching and learning—the rhetorics of the science classroom. London and New York: Continuum.Google Scholar
  22. Lemke, J. L. (2001). Articulating communities: sociocultural perspectives on science education. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 38(3), 296–316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Mathewson, J. H. (1999). Visual-spatial thinking: an aspect of science overlooked by educators. Science Education, 83(1), 33–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Murmann, L., & Schwedes, H. (1999). Learning processes concerning ‘light and shadow’ during science education in elementary school. Research in Science Education-Past, Present, and Future, 1, 110–112.Google Scholar
  25. Ogborn, J., Kress, G., Martins, I., & McGillicuddy, K. (1996). Explaining science in the classroom. Buckingham/Philadelphia: Open University Press.Google Scholar
  26. Osborne, J. F., Black, P., Meadows, J., & Smith, M. (1993). Young children’s (7–11) ideas about light and their development. International Journal of Science Education, 15(1), 83–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Pantidos, P. & Givry, D. (2014). Ambiguities in representing the concept of energy: a semiotic approach. Review of Science, Mathematics and ICT Education, 8.Google Scholar
  28. Pantidos, P., Valakas, K., Ravanis, K., & Vitoratos, E. (2008). Towards applied semiotics: an analysis of iconic gestural signs regarding physics teaching in the light of theatre semiotics. Semiotica, 172(1/4), 201–231.Google Scholar
  29. Pantidos, P., Valakas, K., Vitoratos, E., & Ravanis, K. (2010). The materiality of narrative spaces: a theatre semiotics perspective into the teaching of physics. Semiotica, 182(1/4), 305–325.Google Scholar
  30. Papandreou, M. (2012). My shadow and me: children’s drawing in the context of teaching shadow formation in the kindergarten. In K. Plakitsi (Ed.), Sociocognitive and sociocultural approaches to science in early childhood (pp. 362–375). Athens: Patakis Press (in Greek).Google Scholar
  31. Parker, J. (2006). Exploring the impact of varying degrees of cognitive conflict in the generation of both subject and pedagogical knowledge as primary trainee teachers learn about shadow formation. International Journal of Science Education, 28(13), 1545–1577.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Ping, R. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2008). Hands in the air: using ungrounded iconic gestures to teach children conservation of quantity. Developmental Psychology, 44(5), 1277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Pozzer-Ardenghi, L., & Roth, W. M. (2007). On performing concepts during science lectures. Science Education, 91(1), 96–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Pozzer-Ardenghi, L., & Roth, W.-M. (2010). Staging & performing scientific concepts: lecturing is thinking with hands, eyes, body, & signs. Rotterdam: Sense Publishers.Google Scholar
  35. Rahm, J. (2004). Multiple modes of meaning‐making in a science center. Science Education, 88(2), 223–247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Ravanis, K. (1994). The discovery of elementary magnetic properties in pre-school age. A qualitative and quantitative research within a Piagetian framework. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 2(2), 79–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Ravanis, K. (1996). Stratégies d’interventions didactiques pour l’initiation des enfants de l’école maternelle en sciences physiques. Spirale-Revue de Recherches en Éducation, 17, 161–176.Google Scholar
  38. Ravanis, K., Zacharos, K., & Vellopoulou, A. (2010). The formation of shadows: the case of the position of a light source in relevance to the shadow. Acta Didactica Napocensia, 3(3), 1–6.Google Scholar
  39. Ravanis, K., Christidou, V., & Hatzinikita, V. (2013). Enhancing conceptual change in preschool children’s representations of light: a sociocognitive approach. Research in Science Education, 43(6), 2257–2276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Rennie, L. J., & Jarvis, T. (1995). Children’s choice of drawings to communicate their ideas about technology. Research in Science Education, 25(3), 239–252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Rieber, L. P. (2005). Multimedia learning in games, simulations, and microworlds. The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning, 549–567.Google Scholar
  42. Roth, W.-M. (1998). Situated cognition and assessment of competence in science. Evaluation and Programming Planning, 21, 155–169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Roth, W. M. (2001). Gestures: their role in teaching and learning. Review of Educational Research, 71(3), 365–392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Roth, W. M. (2003). From epistemic (ergotic) actions to scientific discourse: the bridging function of gestures. Pragmatics and Cognition, 11(1), 141–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Roth, W.-M., & Lawless, D. (2002). Signs, deixis, and the emergence of scientific explanation. Semiotica, 138(1/4), 95–130.Google Scholar
  46. Russell, T., Harlen, W., & Watt, D. (1989). Children’s ideas about evaporation. International Journal of Science Education, 11, 566–576.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Segal, G., & Cosgrove, M. (1993). The sun is sleeping now: early learning about light and shadows. Research in Science Education, 23, 276–285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Sharp, J. (1995). Children’s astronomy: implications for curriculum developments at key stage 1 and the future of infant science in England and Wales. International Journal of Early Years Education, 3(3), 17–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Shepardson, D. P., Wee, B., Priddy, M., & Harbor, J. (2007). Students’ mental models of the environment. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 44(2), 327–348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Trend, R., Everett, L., & Dove, J. (2000). Interpreting primary children’s representations of mountains and mountainous landscapes and environments. Research in Science and Technological Education, 18(1), 85–112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Trundle, K. & Hilson, M. (2012). Shadows play. Science and Children, 49(5).Google Scholar
  52. Tsatsaroni, A., Ravanis, K., & Falaga, A. (2003). Studying the recontextualisation of science in preschool classrooms: drawing on Bernstein’s insights into teaching and learning practices. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 1(4), 385–417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Tytler, R., & Peterson, S. (2001). Deconstructing learning in science: young children’s responses to a classroom sequence on evaporation. Research in Science Education, 30(4), 339–355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Tytler, R., Prain, V., & Peterson, S. (2007). Representational issues in students learning about evaporation. Research in Science Education, 37(3), 313–331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Venville, G., Adey, P., Larkin, S., & Robertson, A. (2003). Fostering thinking through science in the early years of schooling. International Journal of Science Education, 25(11), 1313–1331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Vincent, D., & Cassel, D. (2011). Shadows that enlighten. Science and Children, 48(5), 50–54.Google Scholar
  57. Vosniadou, S., Skopeliti, I., & Ikospentaki, K. (2004). Modes of knowing and ways of reasoning in elementary astronomy. Cognitive Development, 19(2), 203–222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Weil-Barais, A. (2001). Constructivist approaches and the teaching of science. Prospects, 31(2), 187–196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Young, T., & Guy, M. (2008). Moon’s phases and the self shadow. Science and Children, 46(1), 30–35.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2015

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of Early Childhood Education, Faculty of EducationAristotle University of ThessalonikiThessalonikiGreece

Personalised recommendations