Abstract
Why do some individuals support nationalist policies while others don’t? The Savanna-IQ Interaction Hypothesis in evolutionary psychology suggests that more intelligent individuals may be more likely to acquire and espouse evolutionarily novel values whereas less intelligent individuals may be more likely to hold evolutionarily familiar values. Nationalism is evolutionarily familiar, so the Savanna-IQ Interaction Hypothesis suggests that less intelligent individuals may be more likely to be nationalist. The analyses of the General Social Survey (GSS) data in the US and the National Child Development Study (NCDS) data in the UK confirmed the prediction. Less intelligent Americans were more likely to have nationalist attitudes, and less intelligent British voters were more likely to support nationalist parties in five general elections over three decades. The tendency of less intelligent individuals to be more nationalist and belligerent may, among other things, form the microfoundation of democratic peace in international relations.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.


Data Availability
All replication materials are available at https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/VCWAXV.
References
Abelson, R. P. (1985). A variance explanation paradox: When a little is a lot. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 129–133.
Acharya, A., Blackwell, M., & Sen, M. (2016). Explaining causal findings without bias: Detecting and assessing direct effects. American Political Science Review, 110, 512–529.
Anderson, B. (1983). Imagined communities: Reflections on the origin and spread of nationalism. Verso.
Bertoli, A. D. (2017). Nationalism and conflict: Lessons from international sports. International Studies Quarterly, 61(4), 835–849.
Bonikowski, B. (2016). Nationalism in settled times. Annual Review of Sociology, 42, 427–449.
Bonikowski, B., & DiMaggio, P. (2016). Varieties of American popular nationalism. American Sociological Review, 81(5), 949–980.
Bueno de Mesquita, B., Morrow, J., Siverson, R., & Smith, A. (1999). An institutional explanation of the democratic peace. American Political Science Review, 93(4), 791–808.
Chagnon, N. A. (1997). Yanomamö. Harcourt Brace.
Citrin, J., Haas, E. B., Muste, C., & Reingold, B. (1994). Is American nationalism changing? Implications for foreign policy. International Studies Quarterly, 38(1), 1–31.
Cochran, G., & Harpending, H. (2009). The 10,000 year explosion: How civilization accelerated human evolution. Basic.
Cotton, S. M., Kiely, P. M., Crewther, D. P., Thomson, B., Laycock, R., & Crewther, S. G. (2005). A normative and reliability study for the Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices for primary school aged children from Victoria, Australia. Personality and Individual Differences, 39(3), 647–659.
Darlington, R. B. (1990). Regression and linear models. McGraw-Hill.
De Vries, C. E. (2018). The cosmopolitan–parochial divide: Changing patterns of party and electoral competition in the Netherlands and beyond. Journal of European Public Policy, 25(11), 1541–1565.
Deary, I. J., Batty, D. G., & Gale, C. R. (2008). Childhood intelligence predicts voter turnout, voting preferences, and political involvement in adulthood: The 1970 British Cohort Study. Intelligence, 36(6), 548–555.
Dixon, W. J. (1994). Democracy and the peaceful settlement of international conflict. American Political Science Review, 88(1), 14–32.
Doyle, M. W. (1983). Kant, liberal legacies and foreign affairs: Part 1. Philosophy and Public Affairs, 12(3), 205–235.
Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Williams, K. D. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science, 302(5643), 290–292.
Funder, D. C., & Ozer, D. J. (2019). Evaluating effect size in psychological research: Sense and nonsense. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 2, 156–168.
Gellner, E. (1983). Nations and nationalism. Cornell University Press.
Gottfredson, L. S. (1997). Mainstream science on intelligence: An editorial with 52 signatories, history, and bibliography. Intelligence, 24(1), 13–23.
Greenfeld, L. (1992). Nationalism: Five roads to modernity. Harvard University Press.
Hassin, R. R., Ferguson, M. J., Shidlovski, D., & Gross, T. (2007). Subliminal exposure to national flags affects political thought and behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(50), 19757–19761.
Hechter, M. (2000). Containing nationalism. Oxford University Press.
Hilbe, J. M. (2007). Negative binomial regression. Cambridge University Press.
Hobsbawm, E. (1990). Nations and nationalism since 1780: Programme, Myth. Cambridge University Press.
Huang, M.-H., & Hauser, R. M. (1998). Trends in black-white test-score differentials: II. The WORDSUM vocabulary test. In U. Neisser (Ed.), The rising curve: Long-term gains in IQ and related measure (pp. 303–332). American Psychological Association.
Hutchinson, J., & Smith, A. D. (Eds.) (1994). Nationalism. Oxford University Press.
Jensen, A. R. (1997). The g factor: The science of mental ability. Praeger.
Jensen, A. R. (1980). Bias in mental testing. Free Press.
Kanazawa, S. (2004a). The Savanna Principle. Managerial and Decision Economics, 25(1), 41–54.
Kanazawa, S. (2004b). General intelligence as a domain–specific adaptation. Psychological Review, 111(2), 512–523.
Kanazawa, S. (2010a). Why liberals and atheists are more intelligent. Social Psychology Quarterly, 73(1), 33–57.
Kanazawa, S. (2010b). Evolutionary psychology and intelligence research. American Psychologist, 65(4), 279–289.
Kanazawa, S. (2012). The intelligence paradox: Why the intelligent choice isn’t always the smart one. Wiley.
Kanazawa, S. (2014a). Intelligence and childlessness. Social Science Research, 48, 157–170.
Kanazawa, S. (2014b). Intelligence and obesity: Which way does the causal direction go? Current Opinion in Endocrinology, Diabetes, and Obesity, 21, 339–344.
Kanazawa, S. (2017). Higher intelligence and later maternal age: Which way does the causal direction go? Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 65, 1884–1887.
Kanazawa, S. (2019). Cognitive function and crossword puzzles: Which way does the causal direction go? International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 34, 1734–1735.
Kanazawa, S., & Hellberg, J. E. E. U. (2010). Intelligence and substance use. Review of General Psychology, 14(4), 382–396.
Kemmelmeier, M., & Winter, D. G. (2008). Sowing patriotism, but reaping nationalism? Consequences of exposure to the American flag. Political Psychology, 29(6), 859–879.
Kohn, H. (1944). The idea of nationalism: A study in its origins and background. Macmillan.
Lowry, R. (2019). The case for nationalism: How it made us powerful, united, and free. Broadside.
MacMillan, J. (2003). Beyond the separate democratic peace. Journal of Peach Research, 40(2), 233–243.
Miller, A. S., & Kanazawa, S. (2007). Why beautiful people have more daughters. Penguin.
Neisser, U., Boodoo, G., Bouchard, T. J., Jr., Boykin, A. W., Brody, N., Ceci, S. J., Halpern, D. F., Loehlin, J. C., Perloff, R., Sternberg, R. J., & Urbina, S. (1996). Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns. American Psychologist, 51(2), 77–101.
Noury, A., & Roland, G. (2020). Identity politics and populism in Europe. Annual Review of Political Science, 23, 421–439.
Ray, J. L. (1998). Does democracy cause peace? Annual Review of Political Science, 1, 27–46.
Rummel, R. (1983). Libertarianism and international violence. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 27(1), 27–71.
Schrock-Jacobson, G. (2012). The violent consequences of the nation: Nationalism and the initiation of interstate war. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 56(5), 825–852.
Shayer, M., & Ginsburg, D. (2009). Thirty years on—A large anti-Flynn effect? (II): 13- and 14-year-olds. Piagetian tests of formal operations norms 1976–2006/7. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 79(3), 409–418.
Shayer, M., Ginsburg, D., & Coe, R. (2007). Thirty years on—A large anti-Flynn effect? The Piagetian test Volume & Heaviness norms 1975–2003”. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 77(1), 25–41.
Smith, T. W., & Kim, S. (2006). National pride in comparative perspective: 1995/96 and 2003/04. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 18, 127–137.
Sundet, J. M., Barlaug, D. G., & Torjussen, T. M. (2004). The end of the Flynn effect? A study of secular trends in mean intelligence test scores of Norwegian conscripts during half a century. Intelligence, 32(4), 349–362.
Tamir, Y. (2019). Not so civic: Is there a difference between ethnic and civic nationalism? Annual Review of Political Science, 22, 419–434.
Teasdale, T. W., & Owen, D. R. (2005). A long-term rise and recent decline in intelligence test performance: The Flynn effect in reverse. Personality and Individual Differences, 39(4), 837–843.
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1990). The past explains the present: Emotional adaptations and the structure of ancestral environments. Ethology and Sociobiology, 11(4–5), 375–424.
van Beest, I., & Williams, K. D. (2006). When inclusion costs and ostracism pays, ostracism still hurts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(5), 918–928.
van Evera, S. (1994). Hypotheses on nationalism and war. International Security, 18(4), 5–39.
Vanhanen, T. (2003). Democratization: A comparative analysis of 170 countries. Routledge.
Wolfle, L. M. (1980). The enduring effects of education on verbal skills. Sociology of Education, 53(2), 104–114.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Kanazawa, S. Possible Evolutionary Origins of Nationalism. Polit Behav 43, 1685–1705 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-021-09741-7
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-021-09741-7
Keywords
- Psychology of international relations
- Genopolitics