Skip to main content
Log in

Political Considerations in Nonpolitical Decisions: A Conjoint Analysis of Roommate Choice

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Political Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Research shows the increasing tendency of partisan considerations to influence decisions outside the context of politics, including residential choice. Scholars attribute this tendency to affective distaste for members of the other party. However, little work has investigated the relative influence of political and nonpolitical factors in these situations—and it has not sufficiently ruled out alternative explanations for these phenomena. Do people mainly choose to socially avoid members of the other party for political reasons, or is partisanship simply perceived to be correlated with relevant nonpolitical considerations? In some settings, political affiliation may serve primarily as a cue for other factors. As a result, studies that manipulate partisanship but fail to include other individuating information may exaggerate partisanship’s importance in these decisions. To address this shortcoming, I assess the impact of political and nonpolitical considerations on roommate selection via conjoint analysis. I find that partisanship strongly influences this social decision even in the presence of nonpolitical-but-politically-correlated individuating information. Partisan preferences are also moderated by roommates’ perceived levels of political interest. Finally, other social traits do matter, but how they matter depends on partisanship. Specifically, partisans report increased willingness to live with counter-stereotypic out-partisans. This suggests that partisan social divides may be more easily bridged by individuals with cross-cutting identities.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Economists describe this distinction as “taste-based” versus “statistical” discrimination (see, e.g., Guryan and Charles 2013). The former refers to discrimination grounded in animus toward an outgroup. The latter refers to the use of group membership to make inferences about some other trait or characteristic: for example, given limited information, an employer might attempt to assess a prospective employee’s projected productivity based on what he believes about the average productivity of other members of their race, gender, or ethnicity.

  2. For example, imagine a respondent who particularly hates country music, but is largely indifferent about politics. This person might report that they are unwilling to live with a potential roommate who is described as a Republican not because of their political views, but because Republicans (stereotypically) tend to enjoy country music. A researcher might regard this reported “social distance” as symptomatic of affective polarization—since it is observationally equivalent—even though it is actually unrelated to politics.

  3. Subjects were undergraduates enrolled in political science courses who were required to participate in research pool studies for course credit.

  4. To enhance experimental realism, the levels for sexual orientation were weighted 80% straight, 20% LGBT.

  5. Pure independents were excluded from this analysis due to the lack of a single outgroup category here; independent leaners were grouped with the party they preferred.

  6. Evidence for an interactive effect is partly contingent on the precise operationalization. There is less to suggest an interactive effect in the binary outcome models; this may be due to the smaller amount of variation in the dependent variable. Regressing these variables on the rating outcome suggests an interaction between out-partisan affiliation and interest, with out-partisans being evaluated more negatively when they are described as more interested in politics.

  7. There is additionally some evidence of an interaction between respondent political interest and roommate partisan affiliation, with out-partisan roommates being evaluated increasingly negatively as respondent political interest increases. However, this interaction is not robust across different model specifications.

  8. Here, stereotypical Republicans had attribute levels of white, straight, evangelical Christian, likes country music, likes hunting and fishing, and values respecting traditions. Counter-stereotypic Republicans had attribute levels of black, LGBT, nonreligious, likes hip-hop, likes theatre/performing arts, and values treating others fairly. Counter-stereotypic and stereotypical Democrats had the same sets of attribute levels, respectively. The remaining attributes were held constant at “somewhat interested in politics,” “somewhat clean and tidy,” “likes to go to parties on weekends,” and “goes to bed at 11 pm.”

  9. In part, we might expect these evaluations to relate to what social psychologists call the “black sheep effect”—i.e., the tendency toward attitude extremity when judging fellow ingroup members. The black sheep effect holds that people will judge likeable ingroup members more positively than comparable outgroup members—and unlikeable ingroup members more negatively than comparable outgroup members (Marques and Paez 1994). Interestingly, this is not entirely what we observe here: while “likable” (i.e., stereotypical) co-partisans are indeed evaluated more positively than counter-stereotypic out-partisans (who are identical in every respect save partisan affiliation), stereotypical out-partisans are still evaluated most negatively, again illustrating the strength of partisan affiliation in interpersonal evaluations.

  10. One possibility, of course, is that exposure to counter-stereotypic out-partisans merely results in subtyping (Hewstone 1994). Such subtyping allows people to preserve their prior stereotypes of—and affect toward—the outgroup as a whole (Kunda and Oleson 1995).

References

  • Abramowitz, A. I., & Webster, S. W. (2016). The rise of negative partisanship and the nationalization of U.S. elections in the 21st century. Electoral Studies, 41, 12–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Abramowitz, A. I., & Webster, S. W. (2018). Negative partisanship: Why Americans dislike parties but behave like rabid partisans. Political Psychology, 39(S1), 119–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Abrams, S. J., & Fiorina, M. P. (2012). ‘The Big Sort’ that wasn’t: A skeptical reexamination. PS: Political Science and Politics, 45(2), 203–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ahler, D. J., & Sood, G. (2018). The parties in our heads: Misperceptions about party composition and their consequences. Journal of Politics, 80(3), 964–981.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arceneaux, K., & Johnson, M. (2013). Changing minds or changing channels? Partisan news in an age of choice. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bansak, K., Hainmueller, J., Hopkins, D. J., & Yamamoto, T. (2017). Beyond the breaking point? Survey satisficing in conjoint experiments. Presented at the annual summer meeting of the society for political methodology, Madison, WI.

  • Bansak, K., Hainmueller, J., Hopkins, D. J., & Yamamoto, T. (2018). The number of choice tasks and survey satisficing in conjoint experiments. Political Analysis, 26(1), 112–119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barber, B. R. (1984). Strong democracy: Participatory politics for a new age. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, B. (2008). The big sort: Why the clustering of like-minded America is tearing us apart. New York, NY: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cho, W. K. T., Gimpel, J. G., & Hui, I. S. (2013). Voter migration and the geographic sorting of the American electorate. Annals of the Association of American Geographers, 103(4), 856–870.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dafoe, A., Zhang, B., & Caughey, D. (2016). Confounding in survey experiments: Diagnostics and solutions. Working paper. Retrieved from http://www.allandafoe.com/confounding.

  • Deichert, M. A. (2016). Partisan identity and the cultural stereotypes of political parties. Presented at the annual meeting of the midwest political science association, Chicago, IL.

  • Druckman, J. N., & Kam, C. D. (2011). Students as experimental participants. In Cambridge handbook of experimental political science (pp. 41–57).

  • Druckman, J. N., Gubitz, S. R., Levendusky, M. S., & Lloyd, A. M. (Forthcoming). How incivility on partisan media (de-)polarizes the electorate. Journal of Politics.

  • Ferro, S. (2014). The percentage of adult americans living with roommates has been surging. Business Insider. Retrieved November 3, 2014, from http://www.businessinsider.com/americans-with-roommates-are-skyrocketing-2014-11.

  • Fowler, J. H., & Kam, C. D. (2007). Beyond the self: Social identity, altruism, and political participation. Journal of Politics, 69(3), 813–827.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerber, A. S., Huber, G. A., Doherty, D., & Dowling, C. M. (2012). Disagreement and the avoidance of political discussion: Aggregate relationships and differences across personality traits. American Journal of Political Science, 56(4), 849–874.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gift, K., & Gift, T. (2015). Does politics influence hiring? Evidence from a randomized experiment. Political Behavior, 37(3), 653–675.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gimpel, J. G., & Hui, I. (2015). Seeking politically compatible neighbors? The role of neighborhood partisan composition in residential sorting. Political Geography, 48, 130–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gimpel, J. G., & Hui, I. (2017). Inadvertent and intentional partisan residential sorting. The Annals of Regional Science, 58(3), 441–468.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, S. C. (2009). Assessing partisan bias in federal corruption prosecutions. American Political Science Review, 103(4), 534–554.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guryan, J., & Charles, K. K. (2013). Taste-based or statistical discrimination: The economics of discrimination returns to its roots. The Economic Journal, 123(572), F417–F432.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hainmueller, J., Hangartner, D., & Yamamoto, T. (2015). Validating vignette and conjoint survey experiments against real-world behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(8), 2395–2400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hainmueller, J., Hopkins, D. J., & Yamamoto, T. (2014). Causal inference in conjoint analysis: Understanding multidimensional choices via stated preference experiments. Political Analysis, 22, 1–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewstone, M. (1994). Revision and change of stereotypic beliefs: In search of the elusive subtyping model. European Review of Social Psychology, 5(1), 69–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, C. T., & Stull, D. E. (1981). Sex differences in effects of social and value similarity in same-sex friendship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41(3), 488–502.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howat, A. J. N.d. Political engagement and perceived value conflict. Working paper.

  • Huber, G., & Malhotra, N. (2017). Political homophily in social relationships: Evidence from online dating behavior. The Journal of Politics, 79(1), 269–283.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huckfeldt, R., Mendez, J. M., & Osborn, T. (2004). Disagreement, ambivalence, and engagement: The political consequences of heterogeneous networks. Political Psychology, 25(1), 65–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hui, I. (2013). Who is your preferred neighbor? Partisan residential preferences and neighborhood satisfaction. American Politics Research, 41(6), 997–1021.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutter, R. R. C., & Crisp, R. J. (2005). The composition of category conjunctions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(5), 647–657.

    Google Scholar 

  • Inbar, Y., & Lammers, J. (2012). Political diversity in social and personality psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(5), 496–503.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iyengar, S., & Westwood, S. J. (2015). Fear and loathing across party lines: New evidence on group polarization. American Journal of Political Science, 59(3), 690–707.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, W. G. (2014). Is there a culture war? Conflicting value structures in American public opinion. American Political Science Review, 108(4), 754–771.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jennings, M. K., & Niemi, R. G. (1981). Generations and politics: A panel study of young adults and their parents. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joiner, T. E. (1994). The interplay of similarity and self-verification in relationship formation. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 22(2), 195–200.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, J. T. (2018). Fitting the stereotype: Partisan/ideological group stereotypes and social identity. Presented at the annual meeting of the southern political science association, New Orleans, LA.

  • Klar, S., & Krupnikov, Y. (2016). Independent politics: How American disdain for parties leads to political inaction. Cambridge University Press.

  • Klar, S., Krupnikov, Y., & Ryan, J. B. (2018). Affective polarization or partisan disdain?: Untangling a dislike for the opposing party from a dislike of partisanship. Public Opinion Quarterly, 82(2), 379–390.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kunda, Z., & Oleson, K. C. (1995). Maintaining stereotypes in the face of disconfirmation: Constructing grounds for subtyping deviants. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(4), 565.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laar, V., Colette, S. L., Sinclair, S., & Sidanius, J. (2005). The effect of university roommate contact on ethnic attitudes and behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41(4), 329–345.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levendusky, M. (2018). Americans, not partisans: Can priming american national identity reduce affective polarization? Journal of Politics, 80(1), 59–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levitan, L. C., & Visser, P. S. (2009). Social network composition and attitude strength: Exploring the dynamics within newly formed social networks. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(5), 1057–1067.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marques, J. M., & Paez, D. (1994). The ‘black sheep effect’: Social categorization, rejection of ingroup deviates, and perception of group variability. European Review of Social Psychology, 5(1), 37–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • McConnell, C., Margalit, Y., Malhotra, N., & Levendusky, M. (2018). The economic consequences of partisanship in a polarized era. American Journal of Political Science, 62(1), 5–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • McPherson, M., Smith-Lovin, L., & Cook, J. M. (2001). Birds of a feather: Homophily in social networks. Annual Review of Sociology, 27, 415–444.

    Google Scholar 

  • Motyl, M. (2014). ‘If he wins, I’m moving to Canada’: Ideological migration threats following the 2012 U.S. presidential election. Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy, 14(1), 123–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Motyl, M., Iyer, R., Oishi, S., Trawalter, S., & Nosek, B. A. (2014). How ideological migration geographically segregates groups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 51, 1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mummolo, J., & Nall, C. (2017). Why partisans do not sort: The constraints on political segregation. The Journal of Politics, 79(1), 45–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Munro, G. D., Lasane, T. P., & Leary, S. P. (2010). Political partisan prejudice: Selective distortion and weighting of evaluative categories in college admissions applications. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 40(9), 2434–2462.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mutz, D. C., & Mondak, J. J. (2006). The workplace as a context for cross-cutting political discourse. Journal of Politics, 68(1), 140–156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicholson, S. P., Coe, C. M., Emory, J., & Song, A. V. (2016). The politics of beauty: The effects of partisan bias on physical attractiveness. Political Behavior, 38(4), 883–898.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pew Research Center. (2014). Political polarization in the American public. https://www.people-press.org/2014/06/12/political-polarization-in-the-american-public/

  • Prati, F., Crisp, R. J., & Rubini, M. (2015). Counter-stereotypes reduce emotional intergroup bias by eliciting surprise in the face of unexpected category combinations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 61, 31–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prati, F., Moscatelli, S., Pratto, F., & Rubini, M. (2018). Multiple and counterstereotypic categorization of immigrants: The moderating role of political orientation on interventions to reduce prejudice. Political Psychology, 39(4), 829–848.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, K. Roommates wanted. Trump supporters need not apply. The New York Times. 10 February 2017.

  • Rom, M. C., & Musgrave, P. (2014). Political bias in grading: Identifying problems, proposing solutions. Journal of Political Science Education, 10(2), 136–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothschild, J. E., Howat, A. J., Shafranek, R. M., & Busby, E. C. (2019). Pigeonholing partisans: Stereotypes of party supporters and partisan polarization. Political Behavior, 41(2), 423–443.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sacerdote, B. (2001). Peer effects with random assignment: Results for dartmouth roommates. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116(2), 681–704.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schofield, J. W., Hausmann, L. R. M., Ye, F., & Woods, R. L. (2010). Intergroup friendships on campus: Predicting close and casual friendships between White and African American first-year college students. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 13(5), 585–602.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sears, D. O. (1985). College sophomores in the laboratory: Influence of a narrow data base on social psychology’s view of human nature. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(3), 515–530.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shafranek, R. M. Forthcoming. Political consequences of partisan prejudice. Political Psychology.

  • Smith, J. A., McPherson, M., & Smith-Lovin, L. (2014). Social distance in the United States: Sex, race, religion, age, and education homophily among confidants, 1985 to 2004. American Sociological Review, 79(3), 432–456.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stroud, N. J. (2008). Media use and political predispositions: Revisiting the concept of selective exposure. Political Behavior, 30(3), 341–366.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vasiljevic, M., & Crisp, R. J. (2013). Tolerance by surprise: Evidence for a generalized reduction in prejudice and increased egalitarianism through novel category combination. PLoS ONE, 8(3), e57106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lelkes, Y., & Westwood, S. J. (2017). The limits of partisan prejudice. Journal of Politics, 79(2), 485–501.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

I thank John Bullock, Jamie Druckman, Laurel Harbridge-Yong, Chris Karpowitz, Teppei Yamamoto, members of the Druckman political science research lab, and three anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments and advice. This research was approved by the Institutional Review Board at Northwestern University. Data and replication code for the analyses presented in this paper can be accessed at https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/GUATS6.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Richard M. Shafranek.

Ethics declarations

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendix

Appendix

Sample Demographics

See Table 6

Table 6 Sample demographic characteristics

Main Results

See Tables 7 and 8.

Table 7 AMCEs corresponding to Fig. 2
Table 8 OLS model corresponding to Fig. 3

Binary Choice Outcome

See Tables 9 and 10

Table 9 AMCEs of roommate traits on preference evaluations, binary outcome
Table 10 Roommate-respondent trait correspondence and preferences, binary outcome

Instrument

  1. 1.

    Counting this quarter, what is your year in school?

    • First year

    • Sophomore

    • Junior

    • Senior, graduating this year

    • Senior, not graduating this year

    • Other

  2. 2.

    Please choose one or more races or ethnicities that you consider yourself to be (mark all that apply)

    • White (1)

    • Black or African-American (2)

    • Hispanic/Latino(a) (3)

    • Asian or Asian American (4)

    • Middle Eastern or North African (5)

    • American Indian or Alaskan Native (6)

    • Hawaiian or Pacific Islander (7)

    • Other (8)

  3. 3.

    Generally speaking, do you think of yourself as a Democrat, Republican, Independent, or what?

    • Democrat (1)

    • Republican (2)

    • Independent (3)

    • Other party (please specify) (4)

  4. 4.

    [strong/weak partisan or closer to which party]

  5. 5.

    Some people seem to follow what’s going on in government and public affairs most of the time, whether there’s an election going on or not. Others aren’t that interested. Where you would place yourself on a scale from (1) you rarely follow what’s going on in government to (7) you follow what’s going on in government and public affairs almost all of the time?

  6. 6.

    Please select your current age (in years).

  7. 7.

    What is your gender?

    • Male

    • Female

    • Other

  8. 8.

    Which of the following terms best describes your religious beliefs?

    • Catholic

    • Mainline Protestant

    • Evangelical Protestant

    • Jewish

    • Hindu

    • Muslim

    • Nonreligious

    • Other

  9. 9.

    Do you identify as lesbian, gay, bisexual, or transgender (LGBT)?

    • Yes

    • No

  10. 10.

    For the next few minutes, imagine that you are filling out a survey that will be used to match you with a potential roommate.

    Which of the following activities do you enjoy? Check all that apply.

    • Theatre/performing arts

    • Doing yoga

    • Watching foreign films

    • Cars and auto mechanics

    • Visiting farmer’s markets

    • Shopping

    • Swimming

    • Hunting and fishing

    • Playing golf

    • Watching sports

    • Reading

    • Playing video games

    • Rock climbing

    • Volunteering

    • Going to coffee shops

  11. 11.

    Out of the following options, what is your favorite genre of music?

    • Hip-hop

    • Country music

    • Jazz

    • Classic rock

    • Electronic

    • Heavy metal

    • Pop

    • Other

  12. 12.

    On a scale from (1) not at all clean and tidy to (7) very clean and tidy, how would you rate your level of personal cleanliness?

  13. 13.

    Which of the following statements best describes you?

    • I like to go to parties on weekends.

    • I like to stay in on weekends.

  14. 14.

    Below is a list of personal values, i.e., things people might consider important in life. Out of these options, which is most important to you?

    • Helping others around you

    • Treating others fairly

    • Following rules and behaving properly

    • Respecting traditions

    • Trying new things

    • Making your own decisions

    • Living in safe, secure surroundings

    • Enjoying life and having fun

    • Being successful and admired

  15. 15.

    Generally speaking, around what time do you prefer to go to bed?

    • 8 pm or earlier

    • 9 pm

    • 10 pm

    • 11 pm

    • Midnight

    • 1 am

    • 2 am or later

  16. 16.

    [CONJOINT TASK] For the next few minutes, we are going to ask you to act as if you were trying to pick a roommate to live with.

    We will describe to you several pairs of potential roommates. For each pair, please indicate your attitudes towards the two potential roommates and which one you would prefer to live with. Even if you aren’t entirely sure, please indicate which of the two you prefer.


    [Tasks 1–10 here]

    Which of these potential roommates would you rather live with?

    • Roommate 1

    • Roommate 2

    On a scale from 1 to 7, where 1 indicates that you would definitely NOT live with this person, and 7 indicates you would definitely live with this person, where would you place…

     

    Definitely would NOT live with

    Definitely WOULD live with

    1

    2

    3

    4

    5

    6

    7

    Roommate 1

           

    Roommate 2

           
  17. 17.

    Which of the following characteristics are most important to you in a potential roommate? Please rank these items in order from most important (1) to least important (10). (To reorder the items in this list, click and drag.)

    • Their sexual orientation

    • Their hobbies

    • Their taste in music

    • Their cleanliness

    • Their social preferences

    • Their political views

    • Their level of interest in politics

    • Their religious views

    • Their personal values

    • Their race/ethnicity

    • Their preferred bedtime

  18. 18.

    Think about each of the following hobbies. Generally speaking, would you say that each hobby is more commonly associated with Democrats, more commonly associated with Republicans, or not more commonly associated with one party over the other?

    • Theatre/performing arts

    • Doing yoga

    • Watching foreign films

    • Cars and auto mechanics

    • Visiting farmers markets

    • Shopping

    • Swimming

    • Hunting and fishing

    • Playing golf

    • Watching sports

  19. 19.

    Think about each of the following genres of music. Generally speaking, would you say that each genre is more commonly associated with Democrats, more commonly associated with Republicans, or not more commonly associated with one party over the other?

    • Hip-hop

    • Country music

    • Jazz

    • Classic rock

    • Electronic

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Shafranek, R.M. Political Considerations in Nonpolitical Decisions: A Conjoint Analysis of Roommate Choice. Polit Behav 43, 271–300 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-019-09554-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-019-09554-9

Keywords

Navigation