Advertisement

Political Behavior

, Volume 38, Issue 4, pp 861–882 | Cite as

Attitude Responsiveness and Partisan Bias: Direct Experience with the Affordable Care Act

  • Katherine T. McCabe
Original Paper

Abstract

This study evaluates the competing influences of motivated reasoning and personal experience on policy preferences toward the Affordable Care Act. Using cross-sectional and panel survey data, the findings reveal that healthcare attitudes are responsive to information that individuals receive through personal experience. Individuals who experienced a positive change in their insurance situation are found to express more positive views toward the health reform law, while individuals who lost their insurance or experienced an otherwise negative personal impact on their insurance situations express more negative views. The results point to personal experience as a source of information that can influence individuals’ preferences. However, although attitudes are responsive to the quality of one’s personal interactions with the healthcare system, the results also suggest that partisan bias is still at work. Republicans are more likely to blame the health reform law for negative changes in their health insurance situations, while Democrats are more likely to credit the law for positive changes in their situations. These motivated attributions for their personal situations temper how responsive partisans’ attitudes are to information acquired through personal experience.

Keywords

Motivated reasoning Healthcare Public opinion Partisanship Self-interest 

Notes

Acknowledgments

Thank you to Markus Prior, Martin Gilens, Amy Lerman, members of the Princeton Behavior Group, and the anonymous reviewers at Political Behavior for their guidance and helpful feedback. Thank you to the Center for the Study of Democratic Politics at Princeton University for financial support. Replication materials can be found at http://dx.doi.org/10.7910/DVN/COZDRV.

Supplementary material

11109_2016_9337_MOESM1_ESM.pdf (380 kb)
Supplementary material 1 (PDF 379 kb)

References

  1. Achen, C. H, & Bartels, L. M. (2012).Why shark attacks are bad for democracy presented in the Center for the Study of Democratic Institutions, Vanderbilt University.Google Scholar
  2. Ansolabehere, S., & Schaffner, B. (2015). Cooperative Congressional Election Study 2010-2014 Panel Study [Computer File]. Release 1: June 10, 2015. Amherst, MA: University of Massachusetts, Amherst [producer]Google Scholar
  3. Arceneaux, K. (2003). The conditional impact of blame attribution on the relationship between economic adversity and turnout. Political Research Quarterly, 56(1), 67–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bartels, L. M. (2002). Beyond the running tally. Political Behavior, 24(2), 117–150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bullock, J. G. (2009). Partisan bias and the bayesian ideal in the study of public opinion. Journal of Politics, 71(3), 1109–1124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bullock, J. G. (2011). Elite influence on public opinion in an informed electorate. American Political Science Review, 105, 496–515.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bullock, J. G., Gerber, A. S., Hill, S. J., & Huber, G. A. (2015). Partisan bias in factual beliefs about politics. Quarterly Journal of Political Science, 10(4), 519–578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., Miller, W. E., & Stokes, D. E. (1960). The American voter. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  9. Citrin, J., & Green, D. P. (1990). The self-interest motive in American public opinion. Research in Micropolitics, 3, 1–28.Google Scholar
  10. Delli Carpini, M. X., & Keeter, S. (1996). What Americans know about politics and why it matters. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
  11. Fiorina, M. P. (1981). Retrospective voting in American national elections. New Haven: Yale, University Press.Google Scholar
  12. Gaines, B. J., Kuklinski, J. H., Quirk, P. J., Peyton, B., & Verkuilen, J. (2007). Same facts, different interpretations: partisan motivation and opinion on Iraq. Journal of Politics, 69(4), 957–974.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Gilens, M. (2001). Political ignorance and collective policy preferences. American Political Science Review, 95, 379–396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Kaiser Family Foundation: January Kaiser Health Tracking Poll. (2014). The Henry J. Kaiser Family Foundation.Google Scholar
  15. Kaiser Family Foundation: Trends Dataset. (2015). The Henry J. Kaiser Family Foundation.Google Scholar
  16. Key, V. O. (1966). The responsible electorate: Rationality in presidential voting, 1936–1960. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 108(3), 480–498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Lenz, G. S. (2009). Learning and opinion change, not priming: Reconsidering the priming hypothesis. American Journal of Political Science, 53(4), 821–837.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Lenz, G. S. (2012). Follow the leader? How voters respond to politicians’ policies and performance. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Lerman, A. E, & McCabe, K. T. (2016). Personal experience and public opinion: Conditional policy feedback and support for public health insurance. Unpublished.Google Scholar
  21. Malhotra, N., & Kuo, A. G. (2008). Attributing blame: The public’s response to Hurricane Katrina. The Journal of Politics, 70(1), 120–135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Mettler, S., & Stonecash, J. M. (2008). Government program usage and political voice. Social Science Quarterly, 89(2), 273–293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Nawara, S. (2015). Who is responsible, the incumbent or the former president? Motivated reasoning in responsibility attributions. Presidential Studies Quarterly, 45(1), 110–131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Nyhan, B. (2010). Why the death panel myth wouldn’t die: Misinformation in the health care reform debate. The Forum 8(1).Google Scholar
  25. Pierson, P. (1994). Dismantling the welfare state? Reagan, thatcher, and the politics of retrenchment. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Prior, M., Sood, G., & Khanna, K. (2015). You cannot be serious: The impact of accuracy incentives on partisan bias in reports of economic perceptions. Quarterly Journal of Political Science, 10(4), 489–518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Rudolph, T. J. (2003). Who’s responsible for the economy? The formation and consequences of responsibility attributions. American Journal of Political Science, 47, 698–713.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Rudolph, T. J. (2006). Triangulating political responsibility: The motivated formation of responsibility judgments. Political Psychology, 27(1), 99–122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Sears, D. O., & Funk, C. L. (1990). Self-interest in Americans’ political opinions. In J. J. Mansbridge (Ed.), Beyond self-interest. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  30. Sears, D. O., Lau, R. R., Tyler, T. R., & Allen, H. M, Jr. (1980). Self-interest vs. symbolic politics in policy attitudes and presidential voting. American Political Science Review, 74, 670–684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Sekhon, J. S. (2011). Multivariate and propensity score matching software with automated balance optimization: The matching package for R. Journal of Statistical Software, 42(7), 1–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Sirin, C. V., & Villalobos, J. D. (2011). Where does the buck stop? Applying attribution theory to examine public appraisals of the president. Presidential Studies Quarterly, 41(2), 334–357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Stoker, L. (1994). A reconsideration of self-interest in American Public Opinion. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Western Political Science Association.Google Scholar
  34. Taber, C., & Lodge, M. (2006). Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs. American Journal of Political Science, 50(3), 755–769.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Taber, C., Lodge, M., & Glather, J. (2001). The motivated construction of political judgments. In J. Kuklinski (Ed.), Citizens and politics: Perspectives from political psychology (pp. 198–226). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Zaller, J. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PoliticsPrinceton UniversityPrincetonUSA

Personalised recommendations