Philosophical Studies

, Volume 175, Issue 5, pp 1217–1241 | Cite as

Character and theory of mind: an integrative approach

Article

Abstract

Traditionally, theories of mindreading have focused on the representation of beliefs and desires. However, decades of social psychology and social neuroscience have shown that, in addition to reasoning about beliefs and desires, human beings also use representations of character traits to predict and interpret behavior. While a few recent accounts have attempted to accommodate these findings, they have not succeeded in explaining the relation between trait attribution and belief-desire reasoning. On my account, character-trait attribution is part of a hierarchical system for action prediction, and serves to inform hypotheses about agents’ beliefs and desires, which are in turn used to predict and interpret behavior.

Keywords

Character Trait attribution Theory of mind Mindreading Bayesian predictive coding 

Notes

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank Peter Carruthers, Georges Rey, Andrew Knoll, Joseph Jebari, and Charles Starkey for comments on drafts of this paper, and Julius Schönherr, Moonyong Song, Shen Pan, Yichi Zhang, Aida Roige Mas, Kalewold Hailu Kalewald, and Casey Enos for helpful discussion. This research was supported by a Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council Doctoral Fellowship (#752-2014-0035).

References

  1. Ames, D. L., & Fiske, S. T. (2013). Outcome dependency alterns the neural substrates of impression formation. NeuroImage, 83, 599–608. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.07.001.Outcome.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Ames, D. R., Flynn, F. J., & Weber, E. U. (2004). It’s the thought that counts: on perceiving how helpers decide to lend a hand. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(4), 461–474. doi: 10.1177/0146167203261890.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Ames, D. L., Fiske, S. T., & Todorov, A. (2011). Impression formation: A focus on others’ intents. The Oxford handbook of social neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  4. Andrews, K. (2008). It’s in your nature: A pluralistic folk psychology. Synthese, 165(1), 13–29. doi: 10.1007/s11229-007-9230-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Andrews, K. (2012). Do apes read minds?: Toward a new folk psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  6. Anscombe, G. E. M. (1958). Modern moral philosophy. Philosophy, 33(124), 1–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Apperly, I., & Butterfill, S. A. (2009). Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states? Psychological Review, 116(4), 953–970. doi: 10.1037/a0016923.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bar, M. (2007). The proactive brain: Using analogies and associations to generate predictions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(7), 280–289. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bar, M., Neta, M., & Linz, H. (2006). Very first impressions. Emotion, 6(2), 269–278. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.2.269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Behrens, T. E. J., Hunt, L. T., & Rushworth, M. F. S. (2009). The computation of social behavior. Science (New York, N.Y.), 324(5931), 1160–1164. doi: 10.1126/science.1169694.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Brass, M., Schmitt, R. M., Spengler, S., & Gergely, G. (2007). Investigating action understanding: Inferential processes versus action simulation. Current Biology. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.11.057.Google Scholar
  12. Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(5), 187–192. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Carey, S. (2009). The origin of concepts. Oxford University Press. https://books.google.com/books?hl=en&lr=&id=J5flK50tDaIC&pgis=1. Accessed 29 April 2015.
  14. Chater, N., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Yuille, A. (2006). Probabilistic models of cognition: Conceptual foundations. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.05.007.Google Scholar
  15. Chaumon, M., Kveraga, K., Barrett, L. F., & Bar, M. (2014). Visual predictions in the orbitofrontal cortex rely on associative content. Cerebral Cortex, 24(11), 2899–2907. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Choi, I., & Nisbett, R. E. (1998). Situational salience and cultural differences in the correspondence bias and actor-observer bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. doi: 10.1177/0146167298249003.Google Scholar
  17. Choi, I., Nisbett, R. E., & Norenzayan, A. (1999). Causal attribution across cultures: Variation and universality. Psychological Bulletin, 125(1), 47–63. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.125.1.47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Christensen, W., & Michael, J. (2015). From two systems to a multi-systems architecture for mindreading. New Ideas in Psychology. doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2015.01.003.Google Scholar
  19. Cimpian, A., & Salomon, E. (2014). The inherence heuristic: An intuitive means of making sense of the world, and a potential precursor to psychological essentialism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37(05), 461–480. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13002197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Clark, A. (2015). Surfing uncertainty: Prediction, action, and the embodied mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  21. Cloutier, J., Gabrieli, J. D. E., O’Young, D., & Ambady, N. (2011). An fMRI study of violations of social expectations: When people are not who we expect them to be. NeuroImage. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.04.051.Google Scholar
  22. Cogsdill, E. J., Todorov, A., Spelke, E. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2014). Inferring character from faces: A developmental study. Psychological Science, 25(5), 1132–1139. doi: 10.1177/0956797614523297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Csibra, G. (2008). Action mirroring and action understanding: An alternative account. In P. Haggard, Y. Rossetti, & M. Kawato (Eds.), Sensorymotor foundations of higher cognition. Attention and performance XXII (pp. 435–459). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  24. Cuddy, A. J. C., Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (2007). The BIAS map: Behaviors from intergroup affect and stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(4), 631–648. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.4.631.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Cuddy, A. J. C., Fiske, S. T., Kwan, V. S. Y., Glick, P., Demoulin, S., Leyens, J.-P., et al. (2009). Stereotype content model across cultures: Towards universal similarities and some differences. British Journal of Social Psychology, 48(1), 1–33. doi: 10.1348/014466608X314935.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Cunningham, W. A., Zelazo, P. D., Packer, D. J., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2007). The iterative reprocessing model: A multilevel framework for attitudes and evaluation. Social Cognition, 25(5), 736–760. doi: 10.1521/soco.2007.25.5.736.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. de Bruin, L., & Strijbos, D. (2015). Direct social perception, mindreading and Bayesian predictive coding. Consciousness and Cognition, 36, 565–570. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2015.04.014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Deen, B., & Saxe, R. R. (2012). Neural correlates of social perception: The posterior superior temporal sulcus is modulated by action rationality, but not animacy. In Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Cognitive Science Society Conference (pp. 276–281).Google Scholar
  29. Doris, J. M. (2002). Lack of character: Personality and moral behavior. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Fein, S. (1996). Effects of suspicion on attributional thinking and the correspondence bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(6), 1164. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.6.1164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Ferrari, C., Vecchi, T., Todorov, A., & Cattaneo, Z. (2016). Interfering with activity in the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex via TMS affects social impressions updating. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. doi: 10.3758/s13415-016-0419-2.Google Scholar
  32. Fiebich, A., & Coltheart, M. (2015). Various ways to understand other minds: Towards a pluralistic approach to the explanation of social understanding. Mind and Language, 30(3), 235–258. doi: 10.1111/mila.12079.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Fiske, S. T. (2015). Intergroup biases: A focus on stereotype content. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 3(April), 45–50. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.01.010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., & Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimensions of social cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 77–83. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Foot, P. (1967). Theories of ethics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  36. Friston, K., & Kiebel, S. (2009). Predictive coding under the free-energy principle. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 364(1521), 1211–1221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the mind-reading. Trends in cognitive sciences, 2(12), 493–501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Gawronski, B. (2004). Theory-based bias correction in dispositional inference: The fundamental attribution error is dead, long live the correspondence bias. European Review of Social Psychology. doi: 10.1080/10463280440000026.Google Scholar
  39. Gelman, S. A. (2004). Psychological essentialism in children. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(9), 404–409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Gilbert, D. T., Pelham, B. W., & Krull, D. S. (1988). On cognitive busyness: When person perceivers meet persons perceived. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(5), 733–740. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.5.733.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Gilbert, D. T., Malone, P. S., Aronson, J., Giesler, B., Higgins, T., Ross, L., et al. (1995). The correspondence bias. Psychological Bulletin, 117(1), 21–38. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.1.21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Goldman, A. I. (2006). Simulating minds: The philosophy, psychology, and neuroscience of mindreading. Oxford University Press. https://books.google.com/books?hl=en&lr=&id=gRlnfe67ZAQC&pgis=1. Accessed 5 May 2015.
  43. Gopnik, A., & Wellman, H. M. (2012). Reconstructing constructivism: Causal models, Bayesian learning mechanisms, and the theory theory. Psychological Bulletin, 138(6), 1085–1108. doi: 10.1037/a0028044.Reconstructing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Gray, C. (2007). Writing social stories with Carol Gray. Arlington: Future Horizons.Google Scholar
  45. Harman, G. (1999). Moral philosophy meets social psychology: Virtue ethics and the fundamental attribution error author (s): Gilbert Harman Source. In Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, New Series, 99 (pp. 315–331).Google Scholar
  46. Harris, L. T., Todorov, A., & Fiske, S. T. (2005). Attributions on the brain: Neuro-imaging dispositional inferences, beyond theory of mind. NeuroImage, 28(4), 763–769. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.021.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Haslam, N., Bastian, B., & Kashima, Y. (2006). Psychological essentialism, implicit theories, and intergroup relations. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 9(1), 63–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Hassabis, D., Spreng, R. N., Rusu, A. A., Robbins, C. A., Mar, R. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2013). Imagine all the people: How the brain creates and uses personality models to predict behavior. Cerebral Cortex, 24(8), 1979–1987. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht042.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Heal, J. (1996). Simulation, theory, and content. In P. Carruthers & P. K. Smith (Eds.), Theories of theories of mind (pp. 75–89). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Hohwy, J. (2013). The predictive mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Hohwy, J., & Palmer, C. (2014). Social cognition as causal inference: Implications for common knowledge and autism. In M. Gallotti & J. Michael (Eds.), Perspectives on social ontology and social cognition (pp. 167–189). Dordrecht: Springer, Netherlands. doi: 10.1007/978-94-017-9147-2_12.Google Scholar
  52. Hooper, N., Ergogan, A., Keen, G., Lawton, K., & McHugh, L. (2015). Perspective taking reduces the fundamental attribution error.pdf. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 4, 69–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Icard, T. (2016). Subjective probability as sampling propensity. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 7(4), 863–903. doi: 10.1007/s13164-015-0283-y.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Isen, A. M., & Levin, P. F. (1972). Effect of feeling good on helping: Cookies and kindness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21(3), 384–388. doi: 10.1037/h0032317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Jacob, P., & Jeannerod, M. (2005). The motor theory of social cognition: A critique. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(1), 21–25. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.11.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Jastorff, J., Clavagnier, S., Gergely, G., & Orban, G. A. (2011). Neural mechanisms of understanding rational actions: Middle temporal gyrus activation by contextual violation. Cerebral Cortex, 21(2), 318–329. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq098.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Jeannerod, M., Arbib, M. A., Rizzolatti, G., & Sakata, H. (1995). Grasping objets: The cortical mechanisms of visuomotor transformation. Trends in Neurosciences, 18(7), 314–320. http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/016622369593921J.
  58. Jones, E., & Harris, A. (1967). The attribution of attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 3, 1–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Jones, M., & Love, B. C. (2011). Bayesian fundamentalism or enlightenment? On the explanatory status and theoretical contributions of Bayesian models of cognition. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34(4), 169–188. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X10003134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York, NY: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  61. Kalish, C. W. (2002). Children’s predictions of consistency in people’s actions. Cognition, 84(3), 237–265. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00052-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Kemp, C., Perfors, A., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2007). Learning overhypotheses with hierarchical Bayesian models. Developmental Science, 10(3), 307–321. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00585.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Kestemont, J., Vandekerckhove, M., Ma, N., Van Hoeck, N., & Van Overwalle, F. (2013). Situation and person attributions under spontaneous and intentional instructions: An fMRI study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(5), 481–493. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss022.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Kilner, J. M., Friston, K. J., & Frith, C. D. (2007). Predictive coding: An account of the mirror neuron system. Cognitive Processing, 8(3), 159–166. doi: 10.1007/s10339-007-0170-2.Predictive.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Kitayama, S., Duffy, S., Kawamura, T., & Larsen, J. T. (2003). Perceiving an object and its context in different cultures: A cultural look at new look. Psychological Science, 14(3), 201–206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Knobe, J. (2003). Intentional action and side effects in ordinary language. Analysis, 63(3), 190–194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Koster-Hale, J., & Saxe, R. (2013). Theory of mind: A neural prediction problem. Neuron, 79(5), 836–848. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.020.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Kovács, Á. M. (2016). Belief files in theory of mind reasoning. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 7(2), 509–527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Krull, D. S., Hui-Min Loy, M., Lin, J., Wang, C.-F., Chen, S., & Zhao, X. (1999). The fundamental fundamental attribution error: Correspondence bias in individualist and collectivist cultures.pdf. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25(10), 1208–1219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Krull, D. S., Seger, C. R., & Silvera, D. H. (2008). Smile when you say that: Effects of willingness on dispositional inferences. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(3), 735–742. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2007.05.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Lakatos, I. (1970). Falsification and the methodology of scientific research programmes. In I. Lakatos (Ed.), Criticism and the growth of knowledge (pp. 91–196). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Liepelt, R., & Brass, M. (2010). Top-down modulation of motor priming by belief about animacy. Experimental Psychology, 57(3), 221–227. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000028.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Liepelt, R., & Von Cramon, D. Y. (2008). What is matched in direct matching? Intention Attribution Modulates Motor Priming, 34(3), 578–591. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.3.578.Google Scholar
  74. Ma, N., Vandekerckhove, M., Baetens, K., Overwalle, F. Van, Seurinck, R., & Fias, W. (2011). Inconsistencies in spontaneous and intentional trait inferences. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(8), 937–950. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr064.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Ma, N., Vandekerckhove, M., Van Hoeck, N., & Van Overwalle, F. (2012). Distinct recruitment of temporo-parietal junction and medial prefrontal cortex in behavior understanding and trait identification. Social Neuroscience, 7(6), 591–605. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2012.686925.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Malle, B. F. (2004). How the mind explains behavior: Folk explanations, meaning, and social interaction. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  77. McCloskey, M., Caramazza, A., & Green, B. (1980). Curvilinear motion in the absence of external forces: Naive beliefs about the motion of objects. Science, 210(4474), 1139–1141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Mende-Siedlecki, P., Cai, Y., & Todorov, A. (2013). The neural dynamics of updating person impressions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(6), 623–631. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss040.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Miller, C. B. (2013). Moral character: An empirical theory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Miyamoto, Y., & Kitayama, S. (2002). Cultural variation in correspondence bias: the critical role of attitude diagnosticity of socially constrained behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.83.5.1239.Google Scholar
  81. Norenzayan, A., Choi, I., & Nisbett, R. (2003). Cultural similarities and differences in social inference: evidence from behavioral predictions and lay theories of behavior. Human Resource Abstracts, 38(2), 109–120.Google Scholar
  82. Onishi, K. H., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science, 308(5719), 255–258. doi: 10.1126/science.1107621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Rakoczy, H. (2017). In defense of a developmental dogma: children acquire propositional attitude folk psychology around age 4. Synthese, 194(3), 689–707. doi: 10.1007/s11229-015-0860-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Reeder, G. D. (2009). Mindreading: Judgments about intentionality and motives in dispositional inference. Psychological Inquiry, 20(1), 1–18. doi: 10.1080/10478400802615744.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Reeder, G. D., Vonk, R., Ronk, M. J., Ham, J., & Lawrence, M. (2004). Dispositional attribution: Multiple inferences about motive-related traits. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(4), 530–544. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.86.4.530.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Rhodes, M., Leslie, S.-J., & Tworek, C. M. (2012). Cultural transmission of social essentialism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(34), 13526–13531. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1208951109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings: Distortions in the attribution process. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60357-3.Google Scholar
  89. Sabini, J., & Silver, M. (2005). Lack of character? situationism critiqued. Ethics, 115(3), 535–562. doi: 10.1086/428459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Sagar, H. A., & Schofield, J. W. (1980). Racial and behavioral cues in Black and White children’s perceptions of ambiguously aggressive acts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(4), 590–598. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.39.4.590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Saxe, R., & Kanwisher, N. (2003). People thinking about thinking peopleThe role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind”. NeuroImage, 19(4), 1835–1842. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00230-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Saxe, R., & Wexler, A. (2005). Making sense of another mind: The role of the right temporo-parietal junction. Neuropsychologia. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.02.013.Google Scholar
  93. Seligman, M. E. P., Railton, P., Baumeister, R. F., & Sripada, C. (2013). Navigating into the future or driven by the past. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(2), 119–141. doi: 10.1177/1745691612474317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. Spaulding, S. (2016). Mind misreading (p. 26). Issues: Philosophical.Google Scholar
  95. Spratling, M. W. (2008). Predictive coding as a model of biased competition in visual attention. Vision Research, 48(12), 1391–1408. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.03.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Spratling, M. W. (2016). Predictive coding as a model of cognition. Cognitive Processing, 17(3), 279–305. doi: 10.1007/s10339-016-0765-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. Sreenivasan, G. (2002). Errors about errors: Virtue theory and trait attribution. Mind, 111(441), 47–68. doi: 10.1093/mind/111.441.47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Sripada, C. S. (2009). The deep self model and asymmetries in folk judgments about intentional action. Philosophical Studies, 151(2), 159–176. doi: 10.1007/s11098-009-9423-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Sripada, C. S. (2012). Mental state attributions and the side-effect effect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(1), 232–238. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.07.008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  100. Sripada, C. S., & Konrath, S. (2011). Telling more than we can know about intentional action. Mind and Language, 26(3), 353–380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  101. Tannenbaum, D., Uhlmann, E. L., & Diermeier, D. (2011). Moral signals, public outrage, and immaterial harms. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.05.010.Google Scholar
  102. Todorov, A. (2013). Making up your mind after 100-ms exposure to face. Psychological Science, 17(7), 592–598.Google Scholar
  103. Todorov, A., Said, C. P., Engell, A. D., & Oosterhof, N. N. (2008). Understanding evaluation of faces on social dimensions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(12), 455–460. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.10.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. Trope, Y., & Gaunt, R. (2007). Attribution and person perception. In M. Hogg & J. Cooper (Eds.), The sage handbook of social psychology (pp. 176–194). London: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
  105. Uhlmann, E. L., Pizarro, D. A., & Diermeier, D. (2015). A person-centered approach to moral judgment. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(1), 72–81. doi: 10.1177/1745691614556679.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  106. Umiltà, M. A., Kohler, E., Gallese, V., Fogassi, L., Fadiga, L., Keysers, C., et al. (2001). I know what you are doing. Neuron, 31(1), 155–165. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00337-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. Van Overwalle, F. (2009). Social cognition and the brain: A meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 30(3), 829–858. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20547.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  108. Wellman, H. M. (2014). Making minds: How theory of mind develops. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  109. Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. Child development, 72(3), 655–84. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11405571.
  110. Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13(1), 103–128. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  111. Zednik, C., & Jäkel, F. (2016). Bayesian reverse-engineering considered as a research strategy for cognitive science. Synthese, 193(12), 3951–3985. doi: 10.1007/s11229-016-1180-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PhilosophyThe University of MarylandCollege ParkUSA

Personalised recommendations