Skip to main content
Log in

Irreducible parallelism in phonology

Evidence for lookahead from Mohawk, Maragoli, Sino-Japanese, and Lithuanian

  • Published:
Natural Language & Linguistic Theory Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

McCarthy (2013) asks whether there are phonological systems necessitating irreducible parallelism in grammar—systems requiring that multiple changes to the input apply in parallel, in a single derivational step. Such systems would necessitate a framework with lookahead: the ability to see from a given derivational step the results of applying multiple changes to its input. This article makes the following claims: (i) a variety of systems across languages, involving a diverse array of processes, require lookahead; (ii) these systems share the same underlying character, despite superficial differences. Our evidence comes primarily from the distribution of stress, lengthening, and epenthesis in Mohawk; reduplication and hiatus repair in Maragoli; syncope and gemination in Sino-Japanese; and assimilation and epenthesis in Lithuanian. All these systems involve what we call a COMPARISON OF PROCEDURES. To best satisfy constraints, the grammar applies one change followed by another, unless the final result is dispreferred. In such a case, the grammar instead applies a different series of changes. We make the argument for lookahead in grammar by comparing the ability of two frameworks—Parallel Optimality Theory and Harmonic Serialism—to capture these systems. We show that Parallel OT captures them naturally, as it permits lookahead and therefore allows the grammar to compare entire procedures. HS, on the other hand, is challenged by them, as it forbids lookahead and thus does not permit the grammar to compare entire procedures unless the changes involved are specified to apply in a single derivational step. That the problem arises in connection with a diverse array of processes suggests that lookahead is not merely the reflex of a single exceptional phenomenon, but rather is a property of the grammar as a whole.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. See analyses of Mohawk stress in Michelson (1988, 1989), Potter (1994), Ikawa (1995), Piggott (1995), Hagstrom (1997), Rowicka (1998), Alderete (1999), Rawlins (2006), Houghton (2013), and Elfner (2016). We use the (1988) transcription system, but follow Michelson (1989), Rawlins (2006), and Elfner (2016) in leaving out allophonic processes like /h/ insertion before /rh/. [ʌ] and [u] are front and back nasal vowels, respectively, [y] a palatal glide, and [ts] an affricate.

  2. We note that [e] does not break up [Cy] and [kw] sequences, despite them violating syllable contact when syllabified apart. In addition, [khy] is cited as a permitted word-initial CCC cluster (as in (6a)), and so epenthesis does not apply to break it up (Michelson 1988). We ultimately leave the extended analysis of Mohawk phonotactics to future research.

  3. A reviewer asks if epenthesis could be reanalyzed as deletion. For example, we could say that the underlying form of the 1P morpheme is /wake/, surfacing faithfully in (/wake-nyak-s/, [wa.ˈken.yaks]) (9a) but undergoing hiatus-avoiding deletion in (/wake-aruʔtat-u/, [wa.ka.ruʔ.ˈtaː.tu]) (6b). This analysis is untenable, as it fails to resolve why [e] deletes even in non-hiatus-environments, where deletion would gratuitously violate syllable structure constraints. If /wake/ were underlying, for example, it would be difficult to explain why the final vowel deletes in e.g. [te.wak.ˈten.yu], *[te.wa.ke.ˈten.yu] (5c), in which no adjacent vowel can motivate hiatus repair. See Michelson (1988) for the complete argument.

  4. Bennett (2012, 2017) and Houghton (2013) present similar analyses of moraic-trochaic languages (Prince 1990; Hayes 1995) in terms of the optimal choice between (ˈH) and (ˈL.L) in different environments.

  5. We say ‘Step 1’ here for expository ease—in fact, we assume that epenthesis applied in a previous step. In Sect. 2.5, we consider an HS-based analysis of Mohawk that abandons the assumption that epenthesis precedes stress.

  6. In order to properly assign violations to long epenthetic vowels, we assume that DepVː can compare the outputs of intermediate mappings to the underlying form. Otherwise, the analysis has no chance of succeeding. See Hauser et al. (2016) for a defense of faithfulness constraints in HS that compare outputs to underlying forms.

  7. Though we do not treat the qualities of the different epenthetic vowels in our analysis, we follow Rawlins (2006) in attributing the difference in quality between [e] & [a] is related to the latter’s appearance at these boundaries. See Rawlins (2006) for an analysis of these facts.

  8. Note that *Non-HdFt/a must make the decision between (40a) and (40b), not Iamb, because AlignR must rank above Iamb to derive the choice of (ˈL.L) over antepenult (ˈH) when [e] occupies an open penult ([(ˈte.ke).riks], *[(ˈteː).ke.riks]).

  9. We refer the reader to Rawlins (2006:32–36) for arguments against Alderete’s (1999) Parallel OT analysis of Mohawk based on HeadDep, and a defense of the approach we outline above, based on the avoidance of long epenthetic vowels. Data that challenge the HeadDep approach for Mohawk in particular involve both [e]-epenthesis (Rawlins 2006:32–33) and minimal word augmentation (Rawlins 2006:36 and Adler 2016:21–22). As it pertains to the latter, for example, avoidance of long epenthetic vowels explains why we see multiple epenthesis, rather than insertion of a long vowel, to resolve monosyllabism (e.g., /s-r-iht-Ø/ → [(ˈi.se)riht], *[(ˈse:)riht] 2A-cook-IMP). HeadDep cannot distinguish between these two alternatives (nor can even a tweaked version of HeadDep targeting open syllables exclusively, as a reviewer brings up). The analysis we adopt can capture the effects without appealing to this constraint. See Elfner (2016:263–265) for other arguments against HeadDep for the analysis of stress-epenthesis interactions.

  10. In hiatus repairs in the language, the surviving vowel undergoes compensatory lengthening unless it is word-final. In the latter case, lengthening is blocked (e.g., (/vi-a/, [vja]) = agr8-of). In the data presented below, the vowel surviving hiatus repair is always final, so compensatory lengthening will play no role in the following discussion.

  11. Tarok (Robinson 1976), Kalanga (Mathangwane and Hyman 1999), and Arosi (Lynch and Horoi 2002) also use reduplication to mark possessive categories.

  12. Prior investigators have enforced reduplicant heaviness either with a constraint that forces it to be heavy, or by specifying that the reduplicant is underlyingly heavy (see Hayes and Abad 1989 and McCarthy et al. 2012b on heavy reduplication in Ilokano, and Blevins 1996 on heavy reduplication in Mokilese—these investigators take one or the other approach). We are agnostic to which analytical choice is superior here, simply taking the latter approach. A merely apparent third alternative approach is to say that length is a remnant of compensatory lengthening following hiatus repair in the base. For example, we could envision the following schematic derivation for [jɔː-j-ɔ]: red-e-ɔ → red-j-ɔː (glide formation with compensatory lengthening) → jɔː-j-ɔː (copying) → jɔː-j-ɔ (final vowel shortening). But [viː-vj-ɔ] and like forms deriving from Ci- prefixes suggest this cannot be the right approach: the copy vowel is long, and yet does not derive from a base vowel that was lengthened at any point in the derivation.

  13. The upcoming OT analysis sets aside certain minor complications in these data. In (/red-o-ɔ/, [wɔː-v-ɔ]), the base prefix undergoes vowel hardening, surfacing as [v] between vowels instead of [w], which can be accounted for with a constraint militating against certain [VwV] sequences (see Zymet 2018 for the analysis). In forms where a Co-prefix was copied (e.g., /red-go-ɔ/, [guː-gw-ɔ]), the reduplicant vowel always surfaces as high, even if the prefix is underlyingly mid. This can be accounted for with a constraint requiring the reduplicant vowel to match its corresponding stem glide for height (as in [g\(_{\mathbf{1}}\)-g\(\mathbf{w}_{\mathbf{1}}\)-ɔ], *[g\(_{\mathbf{1}}\)-g\(\mathbf{w}_{\mathbf{1}}\)-ɔ]; again, see Zymet 2018 for an analysis), or with a constraint against mid-tense vowels whose activity is only observed in environments that are not constrained by input-output faithfulness.

  14. Note that the interaction provides yet more evidence for the emergence of the unmarked (McCarthy and Prince 1994), in the sense that onsetless syllables and consonant-glide onsets are allowed in stems but avoided in reduplicants (cf. Steriade 1988, Hayes and Abad 1989 on onset-skipping).

  15. Though Onset would work just as well for our purposes, see Orie and Pulleyblank (1998) for an argument for NoHiatus in particular for handling hiatus repairs.

  16. Furthermore, candidates such as *[v1ɔː3-v1j23] can be eliminated by a high-ranking constraint enforcing BR-contiguity (McCarthy and Prince 1995).

  17. See Pająk and Baković (2010) for a similar pattern in Polish.

References

  • Adler, Jeffrey. 2016. The nature of conspiracy: Implications for parallel vs. serial derivation, Master’s Thesis, UC Santa Cruz. Rutgers Optimality Archive 1322.

  • Albright, Adam, and Edward Flemming. 2013. A note on parallel and serial derivations. Class notes. Massachusetts Institute of Technology.

  • Alderete, John. 1999. Head dependence in stress-epenthesis interaction. In The derivational residue in phonological Optimality Theory, eds. Ben Hermans and Marc van Oostendorp, 29–50. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, Skye. 2016. Obligatory parallel metathesis in Maltese? Poster presented at the Annual Meeting in Phonology 2016. Los Angeles, California: University of Southern California.

  • Ali, Azra, Mohamed Lahrouchi, and Michael Ingleby. 2008. Vowel epenthesis, acoustics and phonology patterns in Moroccan Arabic. Interspeech 2008, Brisbane, Australia, 1178–1181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baković, Eric. 2005. Antigemination, assimilation, and the determination of identity. Phonology 22(3): 279–315.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bennett, Ryan. 2012. Foot-conditioned phonotactics and prosodic constituency. Santa Cruz dissertation, University of California

  • Bennett, Ryan. 2017. Output optimization in the Irish plural system. Journal of Linguistics 53(2): 229–277.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bermúdez-Otero, Ricardo. 1999. Constraint interaction in language change [Opacity and globality in phonological change]. PhD diss., University of Manchester and Universidad de Santiago de Compostela.

  • Bermúdez-Otero, Ricardo. 2003. The acquisition of phonological opacity. In Variation within optimality theory: Proceedings of the Stockholm workshop on variation within optimality theory, eds. Jennifer Spenader, Anders Eriksson, and Östen Dahl, 25–36. Stockholm: Department of Linguistics, Stockholm University

    Google Scholar 

  • Blevins, Juliette. 1996. Mokilese reduplication. Linguistic Inquiry 27(3): 523–530.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broselow, Ellen. 2008. Stress-epenthesis interactions. In Rules, constraints, and phonological phenomena, eds. Andrew Nevins and Bert Vaux, 121–148. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Casali, Roderic. 1995. Patterns of glide formation in Niger-Congo: An optimality account. Paper presented at the 69th Annual Meeting of the Linguistic Society of America, New Orleans.

    Google Scholar 

  • Casali, Roderic. 1997. Vowel elision in hiatus contexts: which vowel goes? Language 73: 493–533.

    Google Scholar 

  • Casali, Roderic. 1998. Resolving hiatus, New York & London: Garland. :.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam, and Morris Halle. 1968. The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper and Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clements, George Nick. 1990. The role of the sonority cycle in core syllabification. In Papers in Laboratory Phonology I: Between the grammar and physics of speech, eds. John Kingston, and Mary Beckman Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, Stuart, and Seung-Hoon Shin. 1999. The syllable contact constraint in Korean: An optimality-theoretic analysis. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 8: 285–312.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Lacy, Paul. 2006. Markedness: Reduction and preservation in phonology. Vol. 112 of Cambridge studies in linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Downing, Laura J., and Sharon Inkelas. 2015. What is reduplication? Typology and analysis part 2/2: The analysis of reduplication. Language and Linguistics Compass 9(12): 516–528.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elfner, Emily. 2016. Stress-epenthesis interactions in harmonic serialism. In Harmonic grammar and harmonic serialism, eds. John J. McCarthy and Joe Pater, 261–300. Sheffield: Equinox Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Féry, Caroline. 1999. German word stress in optimality theory. Journal of Comparative Germanic Linguistics.

  • Flack, Kathryn. 2009. Constraints on onsets and codas of words and phrases. Phonology 26(2): 269–302.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gouskova, Maria. 2004. Relational hierarchies in OT: The case of syllable contact. Phonology 21(2): 201–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gouskova, Maria, and Nancy Hall. 2009. Acoustics of unstressable vowels in Lebanese Arabic. In Phonological Argumentation. Essays on Evidence and Motivation, ed. Steve Parker, 203–225. London: Equinox.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hagstrom, Paul. 1997. Contextual metrical invisibility. In MIT Working Papers in Linguistics 30. PF: Papers at the interface, eds. Benjamin Bruening, Yoonjung Kang, and Martha McGinnis, 113–181. Cambridge: Department of Linguistics and Philosophy, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, Ivy, Coral Hughto, and Megan Somerday. 2016. Faith-UO: Counterfeeding in harmonic serialism. In Proceedings of the annual meeting on phonology 2014. Linguistic Society of America.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, Bruce. 1995. Metrical stress theory: Principles and case studies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, Bruce, and May Abad. 1989. Reduplication and syllabification in Ilokano. Lingua 77: 331–374.

    Google Scholar 

  • Houghton, Paula. 2013. Switch languages: Theoretical consequence and empirical reality. PhD diss., Rutgers University.

  • Hume, Elizabeth. 1990. Metathesis in Maltese: Consequences for the strong morphemic plane hypothesis. North East Linguistic Society (NELS) 21: 157–172.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ikawa, Hajime. 1995. On stress assignment, vowel-lengthening and epenthetic vowels in Mohawk and Selayarese: Some theoretical implications. In Arizona Phonology Conference (5); Proceedings of South Western optimality theory workshop; Coyote papers, Tucson: Department of Linguistics, University of Arizona.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ito, Junko. 1986. Syllable theory in prosodic phonology. PhD diss., Amherst: University of Massachusetts.

  • Ito, Junko, and Armin Mester. 1996. Stem and word in Sino-Japanese. In Phonological structure and language processing: Cross-linguistic studies, eds. Takashi Otake and Ann Cutler, 13–44. Berlin: de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ito, Junko, and Armin Mester. 2015. Sino-Japanese phonology. In Handbook of Japanese phonetics and phonology, ed. Haruo Kubozono, 290–312. Berlin: de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kawahara, Shigeto, Kohei Nishimura, and Hajime Ono. 2003. Unveiling the unmarkedness of Sino-Japanese. In Japanese/Korean Linguistics, Vol. 12, 140–151. Stanford: CSLI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kimper, Wendell. 2011a. Competing triggers: Transparency and opacity in vowel harmony. PhD diss., Amherst: University of Massachusetts.

  • Kimper, Wendell. 2012. Harmony is myopic. Linguistic Inquiry 43(2): 301–309.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiparsky, Paul. 2000. Opacity and cyclicity. The Linguistic Review 17: 351–367.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kurisu, Kazutaka. 2000. Richness of the base and root fusion in Sino-Japanese. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 9(2): 147–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Labrune, Laurence. 2012. The phonology of Japanese. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leung, E. 1991. The tonal phonology of Llogoori: A study of Llogoori verbs. In Working Papers of the Cornell Phonetics Laboratory, Vol. 6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, John, and Rex Horoi. 2002. Arosi. In The Oceanic languages, eds. John Lynch, Malcolm Ross, and Terry Crowley, 562–572. Richmond: Curzon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mathangwane, Joyce T., and Larry Hyman. 1999. Meeussen’s rule at the phrase level in Kalanga. In African mosaic, ed. Rosalie Finlayson, 173–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, John J. 2006. Restraint of analysis. In Wondering at the natural fecundity of things: Essays in honor of Alan Prince, eds. Eric Bakovic, Junko Ito, and John J. McCarthy, 213–239. Santa Cruz, CA: Linguistics Research Center.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, John J. 2007. Slouching towards optimality: Coda reduction in OT-CC. In Phonological studies, ed. Phonological Society of Japan. Vol. 10, 89–104. Tokyo: Kaitakusha.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, John J. 2008. The serial interaction of stress and syncope. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 26(3): 499–546.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, John J. 2010a. An introduction to Harmonic Serialism. Language and Linguistics Compass 4(10): 1001–1018.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, John J. 2010b. Studying GEN. Journal of the Phonetic Society of Japan 13(2): 3–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, John J. 2011. Autosegmental spreading in Optimality Theory. In Tones and features, eds. John A. Goldsmith, Elizabeth Hume, and W. Leo Wetzels. Berlin: de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, John J. 2013. Irreducible parallelism and desirable serialism. Talk given at the annual meeting on phonology, 2013.

  • McCarthy, John J., Wendell Kimper, and Kevin Mullin. 2012a. Reduplication in Harmonic Serialism. Morphology 22(2): 172–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, John J., Kevin Mullin, and Brian W. Smith. 2012b. Implications of harmonic serialism for lexical tone association. In Phonological explorations: Empirical, theoretical and diachronic issues, eds. Bert Botma, and Roland Noske, 265–297. Berlin: De Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, John. J., and Joe Pater. 2016. Harmonic grammar and harmonic serialism. London: Equinox Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, John J., Joe Pater, and Kathryn Pruitt. 2016. Cross-level interactions in Harmonic Serialism. In Harmonic grammar and harmonic serialism, eds. John J. McCarthy and Joe Pater, 88–138. London: Equinox Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, John J., and Alan Prince. 1986/1996. Prosodic Morphology. Vol. 13 of Linguistics Department Faculty Publication Series.

  • McCarthy, John J., and Alan Prince. 1993a. Generalized alignment. In Yearbook of morphology, eds. Geert Booij and Jaap van Marle, 79–153. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, John J., and Alan Prince. 1993b. Prosodic Morphology: Constraint Interaction and satisfaction, 482. Rutgers University Center for Cognitive Science: Rutgers Optimality Archive.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, John. J., and Alan Prince. 1994. The emergence of the unmarked. In North Eastern Linguistic Society (NELS 24), ed. Mercè Gonzàlez. Vol. 2, 333–379. Amherst: GLSA.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, John J., and Alan Prince. 1995. Faithfulness and reduplicative identity. In UMOP 18: Papers in optimality theory, eds. Jill Beckman, Suzanne Urbanczyk, and Laura Walsh Dickey, 249–384.

    Google Scholar 

  • McConvell, Patrick. 1988. Nasal cluster dissimilation and constraints on phonological variables in Gurundji and related languages. Aboriginal linguistics 1: 135–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mester, Armin. 1994. The quantitative trochee in Latin. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 12: 1–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michelson, Karin. 1988. A comparative study of Lake-Iroquian accent. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michelson, Karin. 1989. Invisibility: Vowels without a timing slot in Mohawk. In Theoretical perspectives on native American languages, eds. Donna B. Gerdts and Karin Michelson, 38–69. Albany: State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murray, Robert W., and Theo Vennemann. 1983. Sound change and syllable structure in Germanic phonology. Language 59: 514–528.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olanike Ola Orie, and Douglas Pulleyblank. 1998. Manuscript, Tulane University & University of British Columbia. Retrievable at http://roa.rutgers.edu/files/290-1298/roa-290-orie-1.pdf.

  • Pająk, Bozena, and Eric Baković. 2010. Assimilation, antigemination, and contingent optionality: The phonology of monoconsonantal proclitics in Polish. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 28(3): 643–680.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pater, Joe. 2012. Serial harmonic grammar and Berber syllabification. In Prosody matters: Essays in honor of Elisabeth O. Selkirk, eds. Toni Borowsky, Shigeto Kawahara, Takahito Shinya, and Mariko Sugahara, 43–72. London: Equinox Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piggot, Glynn L. 1995. Epenthesis and syllable weight. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 13: 283–326.

    Google Scholar 

  • Potter, Brian. 1994. Serial optimality in Mohawk prosody. Chicago Linguistics Society (CLS) 30(1): 347–361.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prince, Alan. 1990. Quantitative consequences of rhythmic organization. Chicago Linguistics Society (CLS) 2: 355–398.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prince, Alan, and Paul Smolensky. 1993/2004. Optimality theory: constraint interaction in generative grammar. RuCCS-TR-2 262: ROA-537.

  • Pruitt, Kathryn. 2012. Stress in harmonic serialism. PhD diss., Amherst: University of Massachusetts.

  • Rawlins, Kyle. 2006. Stress and epenthesis in Mohawk. Qualifying paper, University of California, Santa Cruz. https://sites.krieger.jhu.edu/rawlins/wp-content/blogs.dir/28/files/2013/11/rawlins_mohawk_draft.pdf, last accessed 31 May 2020.

  • Robinson, J. O. 1976. His and hers morphology: The strange case of Tarok possessives. In Studies in African Linguistics, Supplement 6, eds. Larry Hyman, L. Jacobson, and Russell Schuh, 201–209. Los Angeles: Department of Linguistics, UCLA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rose, Sharon. 2000. Epenthesis positioning and syllable contact in Chaha. Phonology 17: 397–425.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthall, Samuel. 1994. Vowel/glide alternation in a theory of constraint interaction, Ph.D. dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.

  • Rowicka, Grace. 1998. On Mohawk ghost vowels: Audibility vs. visibility. In Berkeley Linguistics Society (BLS), Vol. 24, 184–194. Berkeley: University of California.

    Google Scholar 

  • Selkirk, Elisabeth. 1984. On the major class features and syllable theory. In Language sound structure: Studies in phonology, eds. Mark Aronoff and R. T. Oehrle, 107–136. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Selkirk, Elisabeth. 1995. The prosodic structure of function words. In University of Massachusetts Occasional Papers in Linguistics 18: Papers in Optimality Theory, eds. J. Beckman, L. Walsh Dickey, and S. Urbanczyk, 439–470. Amherst: GLSA Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Senturia, Martha. 1998. A prosodic theory of hiatus resolution, Ph.D. dissertation, UCSD.

  • Smolensky, Paul, and Geraldine Legendre. 2006. The harmonic mind: From neural computation to optimality-theoretic grammar. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanton, Juliet. 2020. Gurindji nasal cluster effects as trigger deletion. Journal of Linguistics 56(1): 157–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steriade, Donca. 1988. Reduplication and syllable transfer in sanskrit and elsewhere. Phonology Yearbook 5(1), 73–155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steriade, Donca. 1982. Greek prosodies and the nature of syllabification. PhD diss., Massachusetts Institute of Technology.

  • Stonham, John. 1994. Combinatorial morphology. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tateishi, Koichi. 1989. Phonology of Sino-Japanese morphemes. In University of Massachusetts Occasional Papers in Linguistics, Vol. 13, 209–235. Amherst: GLSA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Urbanczyk, Suzanne Claire. 1996. Patterns of reduplication in Lushootseed, Ph.D. dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.

  • Vance, Timothy J. 1987. An introduction to Japanese phonology, Albany: State University of New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walker, Rachel. 2010. Nonmyopic harmony and the nature of derivations. Linguistic Inquiry 41(1): 169–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, Matthew. 2011. Limits on global rules in optimality theory with candidate chains. Phonology 28(1): 87–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zymet, Jesse. 2018. A case for parallelism: Reduplication-repair interaction in Maragoli. UC Berkeley PhonLab Annual Report 14.

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Eric Baković, Ryan Bennett, Dustin Bowers, Paul de Lacy, Eleanor Glewwe, Bruce Hayes, Mwabeni Indire, Junko Ito, Nick Kalivoda, Hope McManus, Nazarré Merchant, Armin Mester, Alan Prince, Hannah Sarvasy, Brian Smith, Bruce Tesar, Kie Zuraw, Erik Zyman, and audiences at NELS47, AMP2016, and the UCSC Phonology Research Weekend for invaluable input. We single out Eric Baković for encouraging the coauthors to collaborate on this paper. Finally, we are grateful to Mwabeni Indire, for sharing the Maragoli language with us.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

The coauthors share first authorship on this paper.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Adler, J., Zymet, J. Irreducible parallelism in phonology. Nat Lang Linguist Theory 39, 367–403 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-020-09478-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-020-09478-8

Keywords

Navigation