Advertisement

Motivation and Emotion

, Volume 42, Issue 2, pp 258–266 | Cite as

It’s a matter of time (perspectives): shifting valence responses to emotional ambiguity

Original Paper

Abstract

Our stage in life has profound influences on our emotions. A well-established age-related positivity effect is putatively related to time perspectives—older adults have a limited time perspective and a greater motivation to experience positivity than young adults. Ambiguous emotions (e.g., surprised expressions) have both a positive and negative meaning, offering a highly relevant model for examining this developmental trend. Indeed, there are stable, trait-like individual differences in valence bias, or the tendency to interpret surprise as positive or negative, with a developmental trend toward positivity (older adults are more positive than young adults, who are more positive than children). However, little research has determined the extent to which the bias can be shifted. In three experiments, we found that ambiguity ratings were sensitive to time perspectives, even within a population of college students, and that this effect is relatively long-lasting. Results extend socioemotional selectivity theory and demonstrate that our life stage may have profound effects on otherwise stable emotional responses.

Keywords

Emotional ambiguity Individual differences Time perspective Valence bias Aging 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We thank Krupa Patel, Hang Nguyen, Kaitlyn Winter, and Aaron Hoagland for help with data collection.

References

  1. Carstensen, L. L. (1993). Motivation for social contact across the life span: A theory of socioemotional selectivity. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 40, 209–254.Google Scholar
  2. Carstensen, L. L. (1995). Evidence for a life-span theory of socioemotional selectivity. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4, 151–156.  https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.ep11512261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., & Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking time seriously: A theory of socioemotional selectivity. American Psychologist, 54(3), 165–181.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.54.3.165.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Charles, S. T., Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2003). Aging and emotional memory: The forgettable nature of negative images for older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132(2), 310–324.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.132.2.310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Cypryańska, M., Krejtz, I., JaskółOwska, A., Kulawik, A., Żukowska, A., De Zavala, A. G., … Nezlek, J. B. (2014). An experimental study of the influence of limited time horizon on positivity effects among young adults using eye-tracking. Psychological Reports, 115(3), 813–827.  https://doi.org/10.2466/02.PR0.115c28z8.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Davis, F. C., Neta, M., Kim, M. J., Moran, J. M., & Whalen, P. J. (2016). Interpreting ambiguous social cues in unpredictable contexts. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(5), 775–782.  https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw003.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  7. Fung, H. H., Carstensen, L. L., & Lutz, A. M. (1999). Influence of time on social preferences: Implications for life-span development. Psychology and aging, 14(4), 595.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.14.4.595.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Kellough, J. L., & Knight, B. G. (2012). Positivity effects in older adults’ perception of facial emotion: The role of future time perspective. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 67(2), 150–158.  https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbr079.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Kennedy, Q., Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2004). The role of motivation in the age-related positivity effect in autobiographical memory. Psychological Science, 15(3), 208–214.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.01503011.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Kim, H., Somerville, L. H., Johnstone, T., Alexander, A. L., & Whalen, P. J. (2003). Inverse amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex responses to surprised faces. Neuroreport, 14(18), 2317–2322.  https://doi.org/10.1097/01.wnr.0000101520.44335.20.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Kim, H. Y., Somerville, L. H., Johnstone, T., Polis, S., Alexander, A., Shin, L. M., & Whalen, P. J. (2004). Contextual modulation of amygdala responsivity to surprised faces. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(10), 1730–1745.  https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929042947865.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2008). International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical report A-8.Google Scholar
  13. Lundqvist, D., Flykt, A., & Öhman, A. (1998). The Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces-KDEF. CD-ROM from Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Psychology section, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm. ISBN 91-630-7164-9.Google Scholar
  14. Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2003). Aging and attentional biases for emotional faces. Psychological Science, 14(5), 409–415.  https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.01455.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2005). Aging and motivated cognition: The positivity effect in attention and memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(10), 496–502.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.005.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Mather, M., Knight, M., & McCaffrey, M. (2005). The allure of the alignable: Younger and older adults’ false memories of choice features. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(1), 38–51.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.134.1.38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Neta, M., Davis, F. C., & Whalen, P. J. (2011). Valence resolution of facial expressions using an emotional oddball task. Emotion, 11(6), 1425–1433.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022993.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  18. Neta, M., Kelley, W. M., & Whalen, P. J. (2013). Neural responses to ambiguity involve domain-general and specific emotion processing systems. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25(4), 547–557.  https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00363.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  19. Neta, M., Norris, C. J., & Whalen, P. J. (2009). Corrugator muscle responses are associated with individual differences in positivity-negativity bias. Emotion, 9(5), 640–648.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016819.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  20. Neta, M., & Tong, T. T. (2016). Don’t like what you see? Give it time: Longer reaction times associated with increased positive affect. Emotion, 16(5), 730–739.  https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000181.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Neta, M., & Whalen, P. J. (2010). The primacy of negative interpretations when resolving the valence of ambiguous facial expressions. Psychological Science, 21, 901–907.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610373934.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  22. Pruzan, K., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2006). An attentional application of socioemotional selectivity theory in college students. Social Development, 15(2), 326–338.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2006.00344.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Samuelsson, H., Jarnvik, K., Henningsson, H., Andersson, J., & Carlbring, P. (2012). The Umeå university database of facial expressions: A validation study. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 14(5), 372–380.  https://doi.org/10.2196/jmir.2196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Shuster, M. M., Mikels, J. A., & Camras, L. A. (2016). Adult age differences in the interpretation of surprised facial expressions. Emotion, 17(2), 191–195.  https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000234.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Tottenham, N., Phuong, J., Flannery, J., Gabard-Durnam, L., & Goff, B. (2013). A negativity bias for ambiguous facial-expression valence during childhood: Converging evidence from behavior and facial corrugator muscle responses. Emotion, 13(1), 92–103.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029431.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Tottenham, N., Tanaka, J. W., Leon, A. C., McCarry, T., Nurse, M., Hare, T. A., & Nelson, C. (2009). The NimStim set of facial expressions: Judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Research, 168(3), 242–249.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2008.05.006.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  27. Williams, L. M., Kemp, A. H., Felmingham, K., Liddell, B. J., Palmer, D. M., & Bryant, R. A. (2007). Neural biases to covert and overt signals of fear: Dissociation by trait anxiety and depression. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(10), 1595–1608.  https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.10.1595.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Maital Neta
    • 1
  • Tien T. Tong
    • 1
    • 2
  • Daniel J. Henley
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Psychology, Center for Brain, Biology, and BehaviorUniversity of Nebraska-LincolnLincolnUSA
  2. 2.Interdisciplinary Graduate Program in NeuroscienceUniversity of IowaIowaUSA

Personalised recommendations