Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Role of self-focus in the relationship between depressed mood and problem solving

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We investigated the effects of adaptive and maladaptive forms of self-focus—specifically, self-reflection and self-rumination—on the relationship between depressed mood and everyday problem-solving behavior. Although previous research has consistently suggested that self-rumination disturbs problem solving and self-regulatory processes, thereby aggravating depressive symptoms, the association between self-reflection, problem solving, and its emotional consequences has not been demonstrated. Therefore, we assessed whether self-reflection can facilitate the emotion regulation function of problem solving through a daily diary method. Thirty-nine Japanese undergraduate and graduate students recorded daily depressed mood, the most stressful problem encountered each day, and whether they utilized problem-solving behaviors for seven consecutive days. Multilevel model analyses showed that individuals with higher levels of self-reflection reported lower depressed moods after enacting problem-solving behaviors, even if the problem that they had on that day was highly stressful. These results suggest that self-reflection enhances the mood regulation function of everyday problem-solving behavior, and may contribute to mental well-being and resilience to stress.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In regard to the Beck Depression Inventory, our aggregated measure of depressed mood showed a correlation size (r = 0.28) almost compatible to that of the single subscale of depressed mood (r = 0.34). These results do not suggest that the aggregated measure captures depressive symptoms better than the single depressed-mood measure, but we decided to report the results of the aggregated measure for the theoretical and conceptual validity as described in the main text.

  2. In the early survey period, 12 participants responded to the original 21-item BDI including "suicidal thoughts." We calculated BDI scores for the 12 participants by summing 20 items excluding "suicidal thoughts." Hence, two groups responded to the original or modified BDI, but there was no significant between-group difference in BDI scores.

  3. Because u 3j did not converge to a positive value when conducting the analysis about self-reflection, we re-estimated the model, fixing u 3j to be zero.

  4. To test whether the BDI has any interactions with problem-solving behavior, stress, or self-focus, we also added the BDI score and its interaction with self-reflection to the equation as independent variables for the Level 1 intercept and slopes. Furthermore, we estimated an analogous model replacing self-reflection with self-rumination. Consequently, all interactions including the BDI were significant in neither the self-reflection nor the self-rumination model.

References

  • Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 217–237. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, R. J., Goddard, L., & Powell, J. H. (2007). Social problem-solving processes and mood in college students: An examination of self-report and performance-based approaches. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 33, 175–186. doi:10.1007/s10608-007-9169-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, R. J., Goddard, L., & Powell, J. H. (2009). Social problem-solving and depressive symptom vulnerability: The importance of real-life problem-solving performance. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 35, 48–56. doi:10.1007/s10608-009-9286-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ayduk, O., & Kross, E. (2010). From a distance: Implications of spontaneous self-distancing for adaptive self-reflection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 809–829. doi:10.1037/a0019205.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognition therapy of depression. New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1982). Control theory: A useful conceptual framework for personality–social, clinical, and health psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 111–135. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.92.1.111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review, 97, 19–35. doi:10.1037//0033-295X.97.1.19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christensen, L., & Duncan, K. (1995). Distinguishing depressed from nondepressed individuals using energy and psychosocial variables. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 495–498. doi:10.1037//0022-006X.63.3.495.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ciarrochi, J., & Scott, G. (2006). The link between emotional competence and well-being: A longitudinal study. British Journal of Guidance and Counselling, 34, 231–243. doi:10.1080/03069880600583287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • D’Zurilla, T. J., Maydeu-Olivares, A., & Gallardo-Pujol, D. (2011). Predicting social problem solving using personality traits. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 142–147. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.09.015.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fenigstein, A., Scheier, M. F., & Buss, A. H. (1975). Public and private self-consciousness: Assessment and theory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43, 522–527. doi:10.1037/h0076760.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garnefski, N., & Kraaij, V. (2006). Relationships between cognitive emotion regulation strategies and depressive symptoms: A comparative study of five specific samples. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 1659–1669. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.12.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayashi, K. (1988). Study on depressive states of students based on Beck’s cognitive therapy. Student Counseling Journal, 9, 97–107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, N. P., Papadakis, A. A., Hogan, C. M., & Strauman, T. J. (2009). Over and over again: Rumination, reflection, and promotion goal failure and their interactive effects on depressive symptoms. Behavior Research and Therapy, 47, 254–259. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2008.12.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., Krueger, A. B., Schkade, D. A., Schwarz, N., & Stone, A. A. (2004). A survey method for characterizing daily life experience: The day reconstruction method. Science, 306, 1776–1780. doi:10.1126/science.1103572.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kraaij, V., Garnefski, N., & Wilde, E. (2003). Negative life events and depressive symptoms in late adolescence: Bonding and cognitive coping as vulnerability factors? Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 32, 185–193. doi:10.1023/A:1022543419747.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kross, E., Gard, D., Deldin, P., Clifton, J., & Ayduk, O. (2012). “Asking why” from a distance: Its cognitive and emotional consequences for people with major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 559–569. doi:10.1037/a0028808.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuehner, C., & Weber, I. (1999). Responses to depression in unipolar depressed patients: An investigation of Nolen-Hoeksema’s response styles theory. Psychological Medicine, 29, 1323–1333. doi:10.1017/S0033291799001282.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lyubomirsky, S., Kasri, F., & Zehm, K. (2003). Dysphoric rumination impairs concentration on academic tasks. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 309–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyubomirsky, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1995). Effects of self-focused rumination on negative thinking and interpersonal problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 176–190. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.69.1.176.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lyubomirsky, S., Tucker, K. L., Caldwell, N. D., & Berg, K. (1999). Why ruminators are poor problem solvers: Clues from the phenomenology of dysphoric rumination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1041–1060. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.5.1041.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–253. doi:10.1037//0033-295X.98.2.224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, L., & Tesser, A. (1996). Some ruminative thoughts. In R. S. Wyer (Ed.), Ruminative thoughts (pp. 1–47). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNair, D., Lorr, M., & Droppleman, L. (1971). Profile of mood states (manual). San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moberly, N. J., & Watkins, E. R. (2010). Negative affect and ruminative self-focus during everyday goal pursuit. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 729–739. doi:10.1080/02699930802696849.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mor, N., & Winquist, J. (2002). Self-focused attention and negative affect: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 638–662. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.128.4.638.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Myin-Germeys, I. (2001). Emotional reactivity to daily life stress in psychosis. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 1137–1144. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.58.12.1137.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nezlek, J. B. (2011). Multilevel modeling for social and personality psychology. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nezlek, J. B., & Kuppens, P. (2008). Regulating positive and negative emotions in daily life. Journal of Personality, 76, 561–580. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00496.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nezu, A. M., & Ronan, G. F. (1988). Social problem solving as a moderator of stress-related depressive symptoms: A prospective analysis. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 35, 134–138. doi:10.1037//0022-0167.35.2.134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). The role of rumination in depressive disorders and mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 504–511. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.109.3.504.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1991). A prospective study of depression and posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster: The 1989 Loma Prieta earthquake. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 115–121. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.61.1.115.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 400–424. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Okazaki, S. (1997). Sources of ethnic differences between Asian American and white American college students on measures of depression and social anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 52–60. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.106.1.52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2006). Computational tools for probing interactions in multiple linear regression, multilevel modeling, and latent curve analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 31, 437–448. doi:10.3102/10769986031004437.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, J. (1987). Self-regulatory perseveration and the depressive self-focusing style: A self-awareness theory of reactive depression. Psychological Bulletin, 102, 122–138. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.102.1.122.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rivkin, I. D., & Taylor, S. E. (1999). The Effects of mental simulation on coping with controllable stressful events. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 1451–1462. doi:10.1177/01461672992510002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schoofs, H., Hermans, D., & Raes, F. (2010). Brooding and reflection as subtypes of rumination: Evidence from confirmatory factor analysis in nonclinical samples using the dutch ruminative response scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 32, 609–617. doi:10.1007/s10862-010-9182-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegle, G. J., Moore, P. M., & Thase, M. E. (2004). Rumination: One construct, many features in healthy individuals, depressed individuals, and individuals with lupus. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 28, 645–668. doi:10.1023/B:COTR.0000045570.62733.9f.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Şimşek, Ö. F., Ceylandağ, A. E., & Akcan, G. (2013). The need for absolute truth and self-rumination as basic suppressors in the relationship between private self-consciousness and mental health. The Journal of General Psychology, 140, 294–310. doi:10.1080/00221309.2013.831804.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skitch, S. A., & Abela, J. R. Z. (2008). Rumination in response to stress as a common vulnerability factor to depression and substance misuse in adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 1029–1045. doi:10.1007/s10802-008-9233-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J. M., & Alloy, L. B. (2009). A roadmap to rumination: A review of the definition, assessment, and conceptualization of this multifaceted construct. Clinical Psychology Review, 29, 116–128. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2008.10.003.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, T. W., & Greenberg, J. (1981). Depression and self-focused attention. Motivation and Emotion, 5, 323–331. doi:10.1007/BF00992551.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stone, A. A., & Neale, J. M. (1984). New measure of daily coping: Development and preliminary results. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 892–906. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.46.4.892.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Takano, K., Sakamoto, S., & Tanno, Y. (2011). Ruminative and reflective forms of self-focus: Their relationships with interpersonal skills and emotional reactivity under interpersonal stress. Personality and Individual Differences, 51, 515–520. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.05.010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Takano, K., & Tanno, Y. (2008). Development of Japanese-version rumination-reflection questionnaire. Japanese Journal of Personality, 16, 259–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Takano, K., & Tanno, Y. (2009). Self-rumination, self-reflection, and depression: Self-rumination counteracts the adaptive effect of self-reflection. Behavior Research and Therapy, 47, 260–264. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2008.12.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Takano, K., & Tanno, Y. (2010). Concreteness of thinking and self-focus. Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 419–425. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2009.11.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thomsen, D. K., Tønnesvang, J., Schnieber, A., & Olesen, M. H. (2011). Do people ruminate because they haven’t digested their goals? The relations of rumination and reflection to goal internalization and ambivalence. Motivation and Emotion, 35, 105–117. doi:10.1007/s11031-011-9209-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trapnell, P. D., & Campbell, J. D. (1999). Private self-consciousness and the five-factor model of personality: Distinguishing rumination from reflection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 284–304. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.76.2.284.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ward, A., Lyubomirsky, S., Sousa, L., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Can’t quite commit: Rumination and uncertainty. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 96–107. doi:10.1177/0146167202238375.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, E. R. (2008). Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 163–206. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.163.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, E., Moberly, N. J., & Moulds, M. L. (2008). Processing mode causally influences emotional reactivity: Distinct effects of abstract versus concrete construal on emotional response. Emotion, 8, 364–378. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.8.3.364.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, E., & Moulds, M. (2005). Distinct modes of ruminative self-focus: Impact of abstract versus concrete rumination on problem solving in depression. Emotion, 5, 319–328. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.5.3.319.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yokoyama, K. (2005). The guide and example commentary for POMS brief form. Tokyo, Japan: Kanekosyobo.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Masaki Mori.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Mori, M., Takano, K. & Tanno, Y. Role of self-focus in the relationship between depressed mood and problem solving. Motiv Emot 39, 827–838 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-015-9486-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-015-9486-x

Keywords

Navigation