Advertisement

Motivation and Emotion

, Volume 35, Issue 1, pp 23–32 | Cite as

Visual search with animal fear-relevant stimuli: A tale of two procedures

  • Allison M. Waters
  • Ottmar V. Lipp
  • Ranjiv S. Randhawa
Original Paper

Abstract

The present study assessed preferential attentional processing of animal fear-relevant stimuli in two procedures, Search and Interference tasks, which have been suggested to reflect on attentional capture due to the fear-relevance of the stimuli presented. In the Search task, participants (N = 154) searched fear-relevant (i.e., snakes and spiders) and non fear-relevant (i.e., fish and birds) backgrounds to determine the presence or absence of a deviant animal from the opposite category. In the Interference task, the same participants searched for the presence or absence of a neutral target (a cat) when either a snake, spider or no distracter were embedded amongst backgrounds of other animal stimuli. Replicating previous findings, preferential attentional processing of animal fear-relevant stimuli was evident in both procedures and participants who specifically feared one animal but not the other showed enhanced preferential processing of their feared fear-relevant animal. However, across the entire sample, there was no relationship between self-reported levels of animal fear and preferential processing which may reflect on the fact that substantial preferential attentional processing of fear-relevant animals was evident in the entire sample. Also, preferential attentional processing as assessed in the two tasks was not related. Delayed disengagement from fear-relevant stimuli appeared to underlie performance in the search task but not in the interference task.

Keywords

Fear-relevance Visual search Attention Anxiety Attentional bias 

Notes

Acknowledgments

Grant DP0770844 from the Australian Research Council and a Griffith University Research Grant supported this work. Thanks are due to Clare Bell, Jacinda Cadman, and Torgeir Solemdal for assistance with data collection and to Paul Jackson who programmed the task.

References

  1. Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 1–24.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Duncan, J., & Humphreys, G. W. (1989). Visual search and stimulus similarity. Psychological Review, 96, 433–458.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Eastwood, J. D., Smilek, D., Oakman, J. M., Farvolden, P., van Ameringen, M., Mancini, C., et al. (2005). Individuals with social phobia are biased to become aware of negative faces. Visual Cognition, 12, 159–179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Fox, E., Lester, V., Russo, R., Bowles, R. J., Pichler, A., & Dutton, K. (2000). Facial expressions of emotion: Are angry faces detected more efficiently? Cognition and Emotion, 14, 61–92.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Gerdes, A. B. M., Alpers, G. W., & Pauli, P. (2008). When spiders appear suddenly: Spider-phobic patients are distracted by task-irrelevant spiders. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 174–187.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Gilboa-Schectman, E., Foa, E. B., & Amir, N. (1999). Attentional biases for facial expression in social phobia. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 305–318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Hadwin, J. A., Donnelly, N., French, C. C., Richards, A., Watts, A., & Daley, D. (2003). The influence of children’s self-report trait anxiety and depression on visual search for emotional faces. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 432–444.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Horstmann, G., Scharlau, I., & Ansorge, U. (2006). More efficient rejection of happy than of angry face distractors in visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 1067–1073.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Klorman, R., Weerts, T. C., Hastings, J. E., Melamed, B. G., & Lang, P. J. (1974). Psychometric descriptions of some specific fear questionnaires. Behavior Therapy, 5, 401–409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Lipp, O. V. (2006). Of snakes and flowers: Does preferential detection of pictures of fear-relevant animals in visual search reflect on fear-relevance? Emotion, 6, 296–308.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Lipp, O. V., Derakshan, N., Waters, A. M., & Logies, S. (2004). Snakes and cats in the flowerbed: Fast detection is not specific to pictures of fear-relevant animals. Emotion, 4, 233–250.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Lipp, O. V., & Waters, A. M. (2007). When danger lurks in the background: Attentional capture by animal fear-relevant distracters is specific and selectively enhanced by animal fear. Emotion, 7, 192–200.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. LoBue, V., & DeLoache, J. S. (2008). Detecting the snake in the grass. Attention to fear-relevant stimuli by adults and young children. Psychological Science, 19, 284–289.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Lundqvist, D., & Öhman, A. (2005). Emotion regulates attention: The relationship between facial configuration, facial emotion, and visual attention. Visual Cognition, 12, 51–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Mathews, A., & Mackintosh, B. (1998). A cognitive model of selective processing in anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 22, 539–560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Miltner, W. H. R., Krieschel, S., Hecht, H., Trippe, R., & Weiss, T. (2004). Eye movement and behavioral responses to threatenting and nonthreatening stimuli during visual search in phobic and nonphobic subjects. Emotion, 4, 323–339.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Mineka, S., & Öhman, A. (2002). Phobias and preparedness: The selective, automatic, and encapsulated nature of fear. Biological Psychiatry, 52, 927–937.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (1998). A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 809–848.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Öhman, A. (1993). Fear and anxiety as emotional phenomena: Clinical phenomenology, evolutionary perspectives, and information processing mechanisms. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  20. Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001a). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental-Psychology: General, 130, 466–478.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Öhman, A., Lundqvist, D., & Esteves, F. (2001b). The face in the crowd revisited: A threat advantage with schematic stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 381–396.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108, 483–522.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Purcell, D. G., Stewart, A. L., & Skov, R. B. (1996). It takes a confounded face to pop out of a crowd. Perception, 25, 1091–1108.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Rinck, M., Reinecke, A., Ellwart, T., Heuer, K., & Becker, E. S. (2005). Speeded detection and increased distraction in fear of spiders: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 235–248.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  26. Tipples, J., Atkinson, A. P., & Young, A. W. (2002a). The eyebrow frown: A salient social signal. Emotion, 2, 288–296.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Tipples, J., Young, A. W., Quinlan, P., Broks, P., & Ellis, A. W. (2002b). Searching for threat. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 1007–1026.Google Scholar
  28. Waters, A. M., & Lipp, O. V. (2008a). The influence of animal fear on attentional capture by fear-relevant animal stimuli in children. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 39–47.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Waters, A. M., & Lipp, O. V. (2008b). Visual search for emotional faces in children. Cognition and Emotion, 22, 1306–1326.Google Scholar
  30. Waters, A. M., Lipp, O. V., & Spence, S. H. (2008). Preferential processing of animal fear relevant stimuli in children. Australian Journal of Psychology, 60, 112–125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Waters, A. M., Nitz, A. B., Craske, M. G., & Johnson, C. (2007). The effects of anxiety upon attention allocation to affective stimuli. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 763–774.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Williams, J. M. G., Watts, F. N., MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. M. (1997). Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders (2nd ed.). Chichester, UK: Wiley.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of PsychologyGriffith UniversityBrisbaneAustralia
  2. 2.School of PsychologyUniversity of QueenslandBrisbaneAustralia

Personalised recommendations