Motivation and Emotion

, Volume 34, Issue 1, pp 2–14 | Cite as

Achievement incentives determine the effects of achievement-motive incongruence on flow experience

Original Paper


Previous research has shown that incongruence between implicit and explicit achievement motives impairs flow experience. We examined this relationship in a more differentiated manner by arguing that achievement-motive incongruence only exerts negative effects when individuals act in situations in which achievement incentives are present and arouse conflict between the two motives. In non-achievement situations, no negative effects of achievement-motive incongruence on flow experience are expected. Study 1 and Study 2 showed that participants with incongruent implicit and explicit achievement motives reported less flow in achievement- as compared to non-achievement-oriented sport situations. In Study 3, we experimentally manipulated achievement and non-achievement situations. Again, motive incongruence impaired the experience of flow in achievement but not in non-achievement situations.


Achievement motive Motive incongruence Incentives Flow experience 


  1. Baumann, N., Kaschel, R., & Kuhl, J. (2005). Striving for unwanted goals: Stress-dependent discrepancies between explicit and implicit achievement motives reduce subjective well-being and increase psychosomatic symptoms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(5), 781–799.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Beckmann, J., & Heckhausen, H. (2008). Motivation as a function of expectancy and incentive. In J. Heckhausen & H. Heckhausen (Eds.), Motivation and action (pp. 99–136). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Boldero, J., & Francis, J. (2000). The relation between self-discrepancies and emotion: The moderating role of self-guide importance, location relevance, and social self-domain centrality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 38–52.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Brunstein, J. C. (2008). Implicit and explicit motives. In H. Heckhausen & J. Heckhausen (Eds.), Motivation and action (pp. 227–246). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Brunstein, J. C., Schultheiss, O. C., & Grässmann, R. (1998). Personal goals and emotional well-being: The moderating role of motive dispositions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(2), 494–508.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Brunstein, J. C., Schultheiss, O. C., & Maier, G. W. (1999). The pursuit of personal goals. In J. Brandtstädter & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Action & self-development (pp. 169–196). London: Sage Ltd.Google Scholar
  7. Clavadetscher, C. (2003). Motivation ehrenamtlicher Arbeit im Verein Mahogany Hall, Bern [Motivation for voluntary work in the association Mahogany Hall, Bern]. Bern, Switzerland: Abschlussarbeit NDS BWL/UF, Hochschule für Technik und Architektur.Google Scholar
  8. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  9. Csikszentmihalyi, M., Abuhamdeh, S., & Nakamura, J. (2005). Flow. In A. J. Elliot & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 598–608). New York: Guilford Publications, Inc.Google Scholar
  10. Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1987). Validity and reliability of the experience sampling method. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175, 529–536.Google Scholar
  11. Csikszentmihalyi, M., & LeFevre, J. (1989). Optimal experience in work and leisure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 815–822.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Csikszentmihalyi, M., Rathunde, K., & Whalen, S. (1993). Talented teenagers. Cambridge, UK: University Press.Google Scholar
  13. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behaviour. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  14. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227–268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Edwards, J. R. (1994). The study of congruence in organizational behavior research: Critique and a proposed alternative. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 58, 51–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Engeser, S. (2004). Lernmotivation und volitionale Handlungssteuerung: Eine Längsschnittstudie beim Statistik Lernen im Psychologiestudium [Learning motivation and volitional action control]. Dissertation. Institut für Psychologie der Universität Potsdam.Google Scholar
  17. Engeser, S., & Rheinberg, F. (2008). Flow, performance and moderators of challenge-skill-balance. Motivation and Emotion, 32, 158–172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Engeser, S., Rheinberg, F., Vollmeyer, R., & Bischoff, J. (2005). Motivation, Flow-Erleben und Lernleistung in universitären Lernsettings. [Motivation, flow experience and performance in learning settings at university]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 19, 159–172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Gable, S. L. (2006). Approach and avoidance social motives and goals. Journal of Personality, 74(1), 175–222.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Higgins, E. T. (1998). Promotion and prevention: Regulatory focus as a motivational principle. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 30, 1–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Hofer, J., & Chasiotis, A. (2003). Congruence of life goals and implicit motives as predictors of life satisfaction: Cross-cultural implications of a study of Zambian male adolescents. Motivation and Emotion, 27(3), 251–272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hofer, J., Chasiotis, A., & Campos, D. (2006). Congruence between social values and implicit motives: Effects on life satisfaction across three cultures. European Journal of Personality, 20, 305–324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Jackson, D. (1984). Personality research form manual. Port Huron, MI: Research Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
  24. Jackson, S. A., Kimiecik, J., Ford, S., & Marsh, H. W. (1998). Psychological correlates of flow in sport. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 20, 358–378.Google Scholar
  25. Jackson, S. A., & Roberts, G. C. (1992). Positive performance states of athletes: Toward a conceptual understanding of peak performance. The Sport Psychologist, 6, 156–171.Google Scholar
  26. Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1996). Further examining the American dream: Differential correlates of intrinsic and extrinsic goals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 80–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Kehr, H. M. (2004). Implicit/explicit motive discrepancies and volitional depletion among managers. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(3), 315–327.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Kristof, A. L. (1996). Person-organization fit: An integrated review of its conceptualizations, measurement, and implications. Personnel Psychology, 49, 1–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Langens, T. A. (2007). Congruence between implicit and explicit motives and emotional well-being: The moderating role of activity inhibition. Motivation and Emotion, 31, 49–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Langens, T. A., & Schmalt, H.-D. (2002). Emotional consequences of positive daydreaming: The moderating role of fear-of-failure. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 12, 1725–1735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. McAdams, D. P., & Bryant, F. B. (1987). Intimacy motivation and subjective mental health in a nationwide sample. Journal of Personality, 55, 395–413.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. McClelland, D. C. (1980). Motive dispositions. The merits of operant and respondent measures. In L. Wheeler (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 10–41). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  33. McClelland, D. C. (1985). Human motivation. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman.Google Scholar
  34. McClelland, D. C., Atkinson, J. W., Clark, R. A., & Lowell, E. L. (1953). The achievement motive. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. McClelland, D. C., & Jemmott, J. B. (1980). Power motivation, stress, and physical illness. Journal of Human Stress, 6, 6–15.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. McClelland, D. C., Koestner, R., & Weinberger, J. (1989). How do self-attributed and implicit motives differ? Psychological Review, 96(4), 690–702.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Puca, R. M., & Schmalt, H. D. (1999). Task enjoyment: A mediator between achievement motives and performance. Motivation and Emotion, 23, 15–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Rheinberg, F. (2008). Intrinsic motivation and flow experience. In H. Heckhausen & J. Heckhausen (Eds.), Motivation and action (pp. 323–348). Cambridge, UK: University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Rheinberg, F., Manig, Y., Kliegl, R., Engeser, S., & Vollmeyer, R. (2007). Flow bei der Arbeit, doch Glück in der Freizeit. Zielausrichtung, Flow und Glücksgefühle. Zeitschrift für Arbeits- und Organisationspsychologie, 51, 105–115. Flow during work but happiness during spare time. Goals, flow experience and happiness. doi: 10.1026/0932-4089.51.3.105.Google Scholar
  40. Rheinberg, F., Vollmeyer, R., & Engeser, S. (2003). Die erfassung des flow erlebens [Measuring flow experience]. In J. Stiensmeier-Pelster & F. Rheinberg (Eds.), Diagnostik von motivation und selbstkonzept (pp. 261–279). Göttingen: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
  41. Ryan, R. M., Chirkov, V. I., Little, T. D., Sheldon, K. M., Tomoshina, E., & Deci, E. L. (1999). The American dream in Russia: Extrinsic aspirations and well-being in two cultures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 1509–1524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social devolopment, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68–78.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Schmalt, H.-D. (1996). Zur Kohärenz von Motivation und Kognition [The coherence of motivation and cognition]. In J. Kuhl & H. Heckhausen (Eds.), Enzyklopädie der psychologie. Motivation, Volition und Handeln (pp. 241–273). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
  44. Schneider, K., & Schmalt, H.-D. (2000). Motivation [Motivation]. Stuttgart: Kohlhammer.Google Scholar
  45. Schüler, J. (2007). Arousal of flow experience in a learning setting and its effects on exam-performance and affect. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 21, 217–227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Schüler, J., Job, V., Fröhlich, S., & Brandstätter, V. (2008). A high implicit affiliation motive does not always make you happy. Motivation and Emotion, 32, 231–242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Schüler, J., Job, V., Fröhlich, S. & Brandstätter, V. (2009). Dealing with a “hidden stressor”: Emotional disclosure as a coping strategy to overcome the negative effects of motive incongruence on health. Stress and Health, doi: 10.1002/smi.1241.
  48. Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (1999). Goal imagery: Bridging the gap between implicit motives and explicit goals. Journal of Personality, 67, 1–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Sheldon, K. M., & Elliot, A. J. (1999). Goal striving, need satisfaction, and longitudinal well-being: The concordance model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 482–497.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Sokolowski, K., Schmalt, H.-D., Langens, T. A., & Puca, R. M. (2000). Assessing achievement, affiliation, and power motives all at once–The Multi-Motive Grid (MMG). Journal of Personality Assessment, 74, 126–145.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Spangler, W. D. (1992). Validity of questionnaire and TAT measures of need for achievement: Two meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 140–154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Stumpf, H., Angleitner, A., Wieck, T., Jackson, D. N., & Beloch-Till, H. (1985). Deutsche personality research form (PRF) [German personality research from (PRF)]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.University of ZürichZurichSwitzerland

Personalised recommendations