Advertisement

Motivation and Emotion

, Volume 33, Issue 1, pp 25–31 | Cite as

Brainstorming rules as assigned goals: Does brainstorming really improve idea quantity?

  • Robert C. Litchfield
Original Paper

Abstract

In two experiments (n = 264 and 339), I treat brainstorming rules as assigned goals and compare their effectiveness to that of quantity goals as interventions to improve the number of ideas generated by individuals. Controlling for goal commitment, I find that brainstorming rules alone do not convey an advantage over even a vague quantity goal presented alone for enhancing the number of ideas generated in two common tasks. Detailed contrasts revealed that specific, difficult goals were only partially effective on their own, as expected when goal commitment is moderate. However, I find evidence in both studies that brainstorming rules are useful adjuncts to specific, difficult quantity goals. Importantly, their combination was the only consistently effective improvement over both vague quantity goals and brainstorming rules presented alone. I discuss implications for future research adopting a goal-based view of intervention in idea generation.

Keywords

Brainstorming Goals Idea generation Creativity 

Notes

Acknowlegments

I thank associate editor Mark Muraven and the anonymous reviewers for their many helpful comments. I thank Angela Henderson for data coding. An earlier version of Study 1 was presented at the 2007 annual meeting of the Academy of Management.

References

  1. Aiken, L. A., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  2. Brown, V., & Paulus, P. B. (1996). A simple dynamic model of social factors in group brainstorming. Small Group Research, 27, 91–114. doi: 10.1177/1046496496271005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Campbell, D. T. (1960). Blind variation and selective retention in creative thought as in other knowledge processes. Psychological Review, 67, 380–400. doi: 10.1037/h0040373.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Dawson, J. F., & Richter, A. W. (2006). Probing three-way interactions in moderated multiple regression: Development and application of a slope difference test. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 917–926. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.91.4.917.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Diehl, M., & Stroebe, W. (1987). Productivity loss in brainstorming groups: Toward the solution of a riddle. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 497–509. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.53.3.497.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Faul, F., & Erdfelder, E. (1992). GPOWER. Bonn: Bonn University Dept. of Psychology.Google Scholar
  8. Gerlach, V. S., Schutz, R. E., Baker, R. L., & Mazer, G. E. (1964). Effects of variations in test directions on originality test response. Journal of Educational Psychology, 55, 79–83. doi: 10.1037/h0048187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Hyams, N. B., & Graham, W. K. (1984). Effects of goal setting and initiative on individual brainstorming. The Journal of Social Psychology, 123, 283–284.Google Scholar
  10. Klein, H. J., Wesson, M. J., Hollenbeck, J. R., & Alge, B. J. (1999). Goal commitment and the goal setting process: Conceptual clarification and empirical synthesis. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 885–896. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.84.6.885.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Klein, H. J., Wesson, M. J., Hollenbeck, J. R., Wright, P. M., & DeShon, R. P. (2001). The assessment of goal commitment: A measurement model meta-analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 85, 32–55. doi: 10.1006/obhd.2000.2931.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Larey, T. S., & Paulus, P. B. (1995). Social comparison and goal setting in brainstorming groups. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 25, 1579–1596. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1995.tb02634.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Latham, G. P., Erez, M., & Locke, E. A. (1988). Resolving scientific disputes by the joint design of crucial experiments by the antagonists: Application to the Erez-Latham dispute regarding participation in goal setting. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 73, 753–772. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.73.4.753.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Litchfield, R. C. (2008). Brainstorming reconsidered: A goal-based view. Academy of Management Review, 33, 649–668.Google Scholar
  15. Locke, E. A. (1966). The relationship of intentions to level of performance. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 50, 50–66.Google Scholar
  16. Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal-setting and task performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  17. Locke, E. A., Latham, G. P., & Erez, M. (1988). The determinants of goal commitment. Academy of Management Review, 13, 23–39. doi: 10.2307/258352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Mullen, B., Johnson, C., & Salas, E. (1991). Productivity loss in brainstorming groups: A meta-analytic integration. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 12, 3–23. doi: 10.1207/s15324834basp1201_1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Nemeth, C. J., Personnaz, B., Personnaz, M., & Goncalo, J. A. (2004). The liberating role of conflict in group creativity: A study in two countries. European Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 365–374. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Osborn, A. F. (1957). Applied imagination. New York: Scribner’s.Google Scholar
  21. Parnes, S. J. (1963). The deferment-of-judgment principle: A clarification of the literature. Psychological Reports, 12, 521–522.Google Scholar
  22. Paulus, P. B., & Brown, V. R. (2003). Enhancing ideational creativity in groups: Lessons from research on brainstorming. In P. B. Paulus & B. A. Nijstad (Eds.), Group creativity: Innovation through collaboration (pp. 110–136). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  23. Paulus, P. B., & Dzindolet, M. T. (1993). Social influence processes in group brainstorming. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 575–586. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.64.4.575.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Paulus, P. B., Nakui, T., Putman, V. L., & Brown, V. R. (2006). Effects of task instructions and brief breaks on brainstorming. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 10, 206–219. doi: 10.1037/1089-2699.10.3.206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Paulus, P. B., & Yang, H. C. (2000). Idea generation in groups: A basis for creativity in organizations. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 82, 76–87. doi: 10.1006/obhd.2000.2888.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Seijts, G. H., Latham, G. P., Tasa, K., & Latham, B. W. (2004). Goal setting and goal orientation: An integration of two different yet related literatures. Academy of Management Journal, 47, 227–240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Sutton, R. I., & Hargadon, A. (1996). Brainstorming groups in context: Effectiveness in a product design firm. Administrative Science Quarterly, 41, 685–718. doi: 10.2307/2393872.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Wegge, J., & Haslam, S. A. (2005). Improving work motivation and performance in brainstorming groups: The effects of three group goal-setting strategies. European Journal of Work and Occupational Psychology, 14, 400–430. doi: 10.1080/13594320500349961.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Economics and Business DepartmentWashington & Jefferson CollegeWashingtonUSA

Personalised recommendations