Skip to main content
Log in

Investigating Investigators: Examining the Impact of Eyewitness Identification Evidence on Student-Investigators

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Law and Human Behavior

Abstract

This research examined the impact of eyewitness identification decisions on student-investigators. Undergraduates played the role of police investigators and interviewed student-witnesses who had been shown either a good or poor view of the perpetrator in a videotaped crime. Based on information obtained from the witness, student-investigators then chose a suspect from a database containing information about potential suspects and rated the probability that their suspect was the culprit. Investigators then administered a photo lineup to witnesses, and re-rated the probability that their suspect was guilty. Student-investigators were highly influenced by eyewitness identification decisions, typically overestimating the information gained from the identification decision (except under conditions that led witnesses to be very accurate), and were generally unable to differentiate between accurate and inaccurate witnesses.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Another fundamental difference between police officers and jurors is that whereas the latter are laypersons with respect to the criminal justice system, police investigators are professionals, often with substantial prior experience working with witnesses. We return to this issue in the General Discussion, but for present purposes it is moot because the research we report tested mock investigators.

  2. The experimenter was not blind to condition which is not ideal as there is the possibility that the note-taker could influence the participants. However, information was recorded in the same manner for all participants by responding in one or two word answers to each of the questions asked. Also, as indicated in the results section, there were no differences in any of the pre-identification measures to indicate that the experimenter affected any of the responses.

  3. For example, looking only at target present lineups, a measure of accuracy can be obtained by calculating the number of identifications of the suspect divided by the number of identifications of the suspect plus foils, that is, given that the witness makes an identification, what is the probability that he or she identifies the culprit? Witnesses were significantly more likely to identify the culprit in the good view condition (56% of the time) than the poor view condition (20% of the time), z = 1.72, p = .043 (one-tailed). However, we are interested in accuracy when the suspect is present as well as when the suspect is not present as this is the case for concern in the real world. As witnesses were no more likely to make an accurate decision when they had a good view (56% of the time) than when they had a poor view (50% of the time) in target absent lineups, z = 0.31, p = .378, it was decided to collapse across viewing condition for all analyses.

  4. Target presence is not included as a factor here to simplify the results as it was found that Fs < 1 for the main effect of target presence and for all interactions with target presence. In the real world, we wouldn’t know if the culprit was present in the lineup or not; thus, we have collapsed across TP and TA conditions. However, details of these analyses are available from the first author.

  5. See Wells and Bradfield (1998) for an explanation of why a MANOVA is the appropriate analysis here.

  6. Target presence is not included as a factor here to simplify the results as it was found that Fs < 1.3 for the main effect of target presence and nearly all interactions where target presence was included as a factor. For the Target × ID Decision interaction, F(1, 118) = 1.48, MSE = 506.95, p = .226, partial η2 = 0.01, and for the Time × View × Target × ID Decision interaction, F(1, 118) = 2.12, MSE = 192.36, p = .148, partial η2 = 0.02.

  7. We thank an anonymous reviewer for bringing this contribution to our attention.

  8. Statistical comparisons aren’t made between Dahl et al. and Study 2 as different materials were used in the two studies. However, in Study 2, pre-identification probabilities were 63.66% (SD = 20.01%) and 65.35% (SD = 16.26%) for the good view and poor view conditions respectively.

References

  • Boccaccini, M. T., Gordon, T., & Brodsky, S. L. (2003). Effects of witness preparation on witness confidence and nervousness. Journal of Forensic Psychology Practice, 3(4), 39–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brigham J. C., & Bothwell, R. K. (1983). The ability of prospective jurors to estimate the accuracy of eyewitness identifications. Law & Human Behavior, 7(1), 19–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, S., Abbe A., & Larson, R. (2006). Collaboration in associative recognition memory: Using recalled information to defend ‘new’ judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(6), 1266–1273.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dahl, L. C., Lindsay, D. S., & Brimacombe, C. A. E. (2006). Investigating investigators: Examining witnesses’ influence on investigators. Law & Human Behavior, 30(6), 707–733.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kassin, S. M., & Neumann, K. (1997). On the power of confession evidence: An experimental test of the fundamental difference hypothesis. Law & Human Behavior, 21(5), 469–484.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindsay, D. S., Nilsen, E., & Read, J. D. (2000). Witnessing-condition heterogeneity and witnesses ‘versus investigators’ confidence in the accuracy of witnesses’ identification decisions. Law and human behavior, 24(6), 685–697.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lindsay, R. C. L. (1994). Expectations of eyewitness performance: Jurors’ verdicts do not follow from their beliefs. In D. F. Ross & J. D. Read (Eds.), Adult eyewitness testimony: Current trends and developments (pp. 362–384). Cambridge University Press.

  • Lindsay, R. C., & Wells, G. L. (1985). Improving eyewitness identifications from lineups: Simultaneous versus sequential lineup presentation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 70(3), 556–564.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindsay, R. C., Wells, G. L., & O’Connor, F. J. (1989). Mock-juror belief of accurate and inaccurate eyewitnesses: A replication and extension. Law & Human Behavior, 13(3), 333–339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindsay, R. C., Wells, G. L., & Rumpel, C. M. (1981). Can people detect eyewitness-identification accuracy within and across situations? Journal of Applied Psychology, 66(1), 79–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Masson, M. E. J., & Loftus, G. R. (2003). Using confidence intervals for graphically based data interpretation. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57(3), 203–220.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. (2000). Eyewitness testimony. Encyclopedia of psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 308–310). American Psychological Association.

  • Wells, G. L., & Bradfield, A. L. (1998). ‘Good, you identified the suspect’: Feedback to eyewitnesses distorts their reports of the witnessing experience. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83(3), 360–376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G., Ferguson, T., & Lindsay, R. (1981). The tractability of eyewitness confidence and its implications for triers of fact. Journal of Applied Psychology, 66(6), 688–696.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. L., Lindsay, R. C., & Tousignant, J. P. (1980). Effects of expert psychological advice on human performance in judging the validity of eyewitness testimony. Law and human behavior, 4(4), 275–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. L., & Olson, E. A. (2002). Eyewitness identification: Information gain from incriminating and exonerating behaviors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 155–167.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yuille, J., & Cutshall, J. (1989). Analysis of the statements of victims, witnesses and suspects. Credibility assessment (pp. 175–191). Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported in part by grants from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada to Melissa A. Boyce, D. Stephen Lindsay and C. A. E. Brimacombe.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Melissa A. Boyce.

Additional information

Portions of this research were presented at the 2005 and 2006 meetings of the American Psychology-Law Society.

Appendix: Equations for Posterior Probability that the Suspect is the Culprit given Eyewitness Identification Decision

Appendix: Equations for Posterior Probability that the Suspect is the Culprit given Eyewitness Identification Decision

$$ p{\text{(S is culprit | IDS)}}\quad \frac{{p{\text{(IDS | S is culprit)}}p{\text{(S is culprit)}}}} {{p{\text{(IDS | S is culprit)}}p{\text{(S is culprit) + }}p{\text{(IDS | S not culprit)}}p{\text{(S not culprit)}}}} $$
$$ p{\text{(S is culprit | IDfoil)}}\quad \frac{{p{\text{(IDfoil | S is culprit)}}p{\text{(S is culprit)}}}} {{p{\text{(IDfoil | S is culprit)}}p{\text{(S is culprit) + }}p{\text{(IDfoil | S not culprit)}}p{\text{(S not culprit)}}}} $$
$$ p{\text{(S is culprit | noID)}}\quad \frac{{p{\text{(noID | S is culprit)}}p{\text{(S is culprit)}}}} {{p{\text{(noID | S is culprit)}}p{\text{(S is culprit) + }}p{\text{(noID | S not culprit)}}p{\text{(S not culprit)}}}} $$

Note: S = Suspect, IDS = identification of suspect, IDfoil = identification of foil, noID = no identification made.

About this article

Cite this article

Boyce, M.A., Lindsay, D.S. & Brimacombe, C.A.E. Investigating Investigators: Examining the Impact of Eyewitness Identification Evidence on Student-Investigators. Law Hum Behav 32, 439–453 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-007-9125-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-007-9125-5

Keywords

Navigation