Advertisement

Journal of Psycholinguistic Research

, Volume 47, Issue 4, pp 931–945 | Cite as

Motivation Among Pre-intermediate Iranian Language Learners: From Explicit Knowledge Toward Implicit Syntactic Development

  • Amirabbas Rafiee Fazel
  • Mehdi Latifi
  • M. Raouf Moini
Article
  • 74 Downloads

Abstract

The variable success in learning a second language (L2) may be best explained by varying degrees of motivation. This study investigated whether individual differences in appraisal dimensions of motivation explained explicit and implicit syntactic knowledge. Participants learned three syntactic structures under implicit and explicit training conditions. They also completed two motivation questionnaires before instruction. Syntactic development was assessed at early and late stages of acquisition. Results indicated superior performance on the tests of explicit knowledge, but a limited effect for the role of motivation in its development. However, with improvement in implicit knowledge at delayed testing, positive relationships emerged with several motivational dimensions. The results support L2 theoretical perspectives that posit different roles for explicit and implicit knowledge at early and late stages of L2 grammar, respectively. The findings also indicate that variation in learners’ patterns of stimulus appraisals may contribute to variable success in the development of implicit knowledge.

Keywords

Explicit knowledge Implicit knowledge Motivation Foreign language Individual differences 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We gratefully acknowledge collaborations by Ali Salehnia and the teachers in the language institute where the data were collected. The authors also thank all of the participants who voluntarily took part in this study.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Ashby, F. G., Turner, B. O., & Horvitz, J. C. (2010). Cortical and basal ganglia contributions to habit learning and automaticity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(5), 208–215.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  2. Bialystok, E., & Fröhlich, M. (1978). Variables of classroom achievement in second language learning. The Modern Language Journal, 62(7), 327–336.Google Scholar
  3. Birdsong, D. (2006). Age and second language acquisition and processing: A selective overview. In M. Gullberg & P. Indefrey (Eds.), The cognitive neuroscience of second language acquisition (pp. 9–49). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  4. Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155–159.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Depue, R. A., & Morrone-Strupinsky, J. V. (2005). A neurobehavioral model of affiliative bonding: Implications for conceptualizing a human trait of affiliation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(3), 313–395.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Dörnyei, Z. (1998). Motivation in second and foreign language learning. Language Teaching, 31(03), 117–135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Dörnyei, Z. (2005). The psychology of the language learner: Individual differences in second language acquisition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  8. Dörnyei, Z. (2009a). The L2 motivational self system. In Z. Dörnyei & E. Ushioda (Eds.), Motivation, language identities and the L2 self (pp. 9–42). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Dörnyei, Z. (2009b). The psychology of second language acquisition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  10. Dörnyei, Z., & Csizér, K. (2002). Some dynamics of language attitudes and motivation: Results of a longitudinal nationwide survey. Applied Linguistics, 23(4), 421–462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Dörnyei, Z., & Ushioda, E. (2009). Motivation, language identity and the L2 self. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Edelman, G. M. (1992). Bright air brilliant fire: On the matter of mind. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  13. Ehrman, M. E., & Oxford, R. L. (1995). Cognition plus: Correlates of language learning success. The Modern Language Journal, 79(1), 67–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Eichenbaum, H., & Cohen, N. J. (2001). From conditioning to conscious recollection: Memory systems of the brain. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  15. Ellis, N. C. (2002). Frequency effects in language processing: A review with implications for theories of implicit and explicit language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24(2), 143–188.Google Scholar
  16. Ellis, N. C. (2005). At the interface: Dynamic interactions of explicit and implicit language knowledge. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27(2), 305–352.Google Scholar
  17. Ellis, R. (2003). Task-based language learning and teaching. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  18. Ellis, R. (2004). The definition and measurement of explicit knowledge. Language Learning, 54(2), 227–275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Ellis, R. (2005). Measuring implicit and explicit knowledge of a second language: A psychometric study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27(02), 141–172.Google Scholar
  20. Ellis, R. (2009a). Implicit and explicit learning, knowledge and instruction. In R. Ellis, S. Loewen, C. Elder, R. Erlam, J. Philp, & H. Reinders (Eds.), Implicit and explicit knowledge in second language learning, testing and teaching (pp. 3–25). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Ellis, R. (2009b). Measuring implicit and explicit knowledge of a second language. In R. Ellis, et al. (Eds.), Implicit and explicit knowledge in second language learning, testing and teaching (pp. 31–64). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2015). DMDX (Version 5.1.3.4) [Computer program]. Retrieved from http://www.u.arizona.edu/~kforster/dmdx/dmdx.htm.
  23. Gardner, R. C. (1985). Social psychology and second language learning: The role of attitudes and motivation. London: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
  24. Gardner, R. C., & Lambert, W. E. (1959). Motivation variables in second language acquisition. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 13, 266–272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Gardner, R. C., & Lambert, W. E. (1972). Attitudes and motivation in second language learning. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
  26. Gardner, R. C., Smythe, P. C., & Brunet, G. R. (1977). Intensive second language study: Effects on attitudes, motivation and French achievement. Language Learning, 27(2), 243–261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hulstijn, J. H. (2005). Theoretical and empirical issues in the study of implicit and explicit second-language learning: Introduction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27(02), 129–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Kensinger, E. A. (2009). Phases of influence: How emotion modulates the formation and retrieval of declarative memories. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (4th ed., pp. 725–737). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  29. Krashen, S. (1982). Principles and practice in second language acquisition. Oxford: Pergamon.Google Scholar
  30. Lee, N. (2004). The neurobiology of procedural memory. In J. H. Schumann, et al. (Eds.), The neurobioloy of learning: Perspectives from second language acquisition (pp. 23–42). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  31. Lee, N., Mikesell, L., Joaquin, A. D. L., Mates, A. W., & Schumann, J. H. (2009). The interactional instinct: The evolution and acquisition of language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Luciana, M. (2001). Dopamine-opiate modulations of reward seeking behavior: Implications for the functional assessment of prefrontal development. In C. A. Nelson & M. Luciana (Eds.), Handbook of developmental cognitive neuroscience (pp. 647–662). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  33. Masgoret, A.-M., & Gardner, R. C. (2003). Attitudes, motivation, and second language learning: A meta-analysis of studies conducted by Gardner and associates. Language Learning, 53(1), 123–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. McLaughlin, B. (1990). Restructuring. Applied Linguistics, 11(2), 113–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Morgan-Short, K., Faretta-Stutenberg, M., Brill-Schuetz, K. A., Carpenter, H., & Wong, P. C. M. (2014). Declarative and procedural memory as individual differences in second language acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(01), 56–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Paradis, M. (1994). Neurolinguistic aspects of implicit and explicit memory: Implications for bilingualism and SLA. In N. C. Ellis (Ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of languages (pp. 393–419). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  37. Paradis, M. (2004). A neurolinguistic theory of bilingualism. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Paradis, M. (2009). Declarative and procedural determinants of second languages (Vol. 40). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Porges, S. W. (2003). Social engagement and attachment: A phylogenetic perspective. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1008(1), 31–47.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Reber, A. S., & Allen, R. (2000). Individual differences in implicit learning: Implications for the evolution of consciousness. In R. Kunzendorf & B. Wallace (Eds.), Individual differences in conscious experience (pp. 227–250). Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Roberts, L., & Meyer, A. (2012). Individual differences in second language learning: Introduction. Language Learning, 62, 1–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Robinson, P. (2002). Theorising and updating aptitude. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Individual differences and instructed language learning (pp. 69–93). Philadelphia: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Scherer, K. R. (1984). Emotion as a multi-component process: A model and some cross-cultural data. In P. Sharer (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 37–63)., Emotions, relationships and health. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  44. Schuchert, S. A. (2004). The neurobiology of attention. In J. H. Schumann, et al. (Eds.), The neurobioloy of learning: Perspectives from second language acquisition (pp. 23–42). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  45. Schumann, J. H. (1997). The neurobiology of affect in language learning. Taunton, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  46. Schumann, J. H. (2010). Applied linguistics and the neurobiology of language. In R. B. Kaplan (Ed.), The oxford handbook of applied linguistics (2nd ed., pp. 244–259). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  47. Schumann, J. H., & Wood, L. A. (2004). The neurobiology of motivation. In J. H. Schumann, et al. (Eds.), The neurobioloy of learning: Perspectives from second language acquisition (pp. 23–42). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  48. Schumann, J. H., Crowell, S. E., Jones, N. E., Lee, N., Schuchert, S. A., & Wood, L. A. (2004). The neurobiology of learning: Perspectives from second language acquisition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  49. Serafini, E. J. (2013). Cognitive and psychosocial factors in the long-term development of implicit and explicit second language knowledge in adult learners of Spanish at increasing proficiency. Doctoral dissertation. Retrieved from https://repository.library.georgetown.edu/handle/10822/559512.
  50. Sorace, A. (1996). The use of acceptability judgments in second language acquisition research. In W. Ritchie & T. Bhatia (Eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 375–409). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  51. Ullman, M. T. (2001). The neural basis of lexicon and grammar in first and second language: The declarative/procedural model. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 4, 105–122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Ullman, M. T. (2004). Contributions of memory circuits to language: The declarative/procedural model. Cognition, 92, 231–270.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Ullman, M. T. (2005). A cognitive neuroscience perspective on second language acquisition: The declarative/procedural model. In C. Sanz (Ed.), Processing approaches to adult SLA: Theory and practice (pp. 141–178). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
  54. Zhang, R. (2013). Investigating linguistic knowledge of a second language and its relationship to general language proficiency and individual learner differences in an EFL context. Doctoral dissertation. Retrieved from https://researchspace.auckland.ac.nz/handle/2292/20499.

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Faculty of Literature and Foreign Languages, English DepartmentUniversity of KashanKashanIran

Personalised recommendations