Advertisement

Journal of Psycholinguistic Research

, Volume 44, Issue 6, pp 803–817 | Cite as

Cognitive Aspects of Regularity Exhibit When Neighborhood Disappears

  • Sau-Chin Chen
  • Jon-Fan Hu
Article

Abstract

Although regularity refers to the compatibility between pronunciation of character and sound of phonetic component, it has been suggested as being part of consistency, which is defined by neighborhood characteristics. Two experiments demonstrate how regularity effect is amplified or reduced by neighborhood characteristics and reveals the regularity effect without the confounding of neighboring characters. Experiment 1 examined the regularity effects of characters with more than one neighboring character, the results of which indicated that the consistency of irregular characters decides the patterns of regularity effects. Experiment 2 included characters without neighboring characters and measured the interaction of character frequency and regularity effect. The interactions of the characters with neighboring characters were reduced by the neighborhood characteristics of irregular characters, but a marginal interaction was found for characters without neighboring characters. The theoretical implication that regularity is acquired from lexical knowledge of phonetic components rather than accumulated from neighboring characters is discussed.

Keywords

Regularity effect Phonetic component Neighborhood characteristics Naming latency 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by Grants from the National Science Council, Taiwan (NSC 98-2410-H-320-009, NSC 99-2410-H-320-009). Jon-fan Hu contributed much effort to commenting on the computational models in the latest version. I thank Jenn-Yeu Chen, Train-Min Chen, and the anonymous reviewers for their valuable comments and suggestions.

References

  1. Andrews, S., Woollams, A., & Bond, R. (2005). Spelling-sound typicality only affects words with digraphs: Further qualifications to the generality of the regularity effect on word naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 567–593.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Baayen, R., Piepenbrock, R., & Van Rijn, H. (1993). The celex lexical database (CD-ROM). Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania, Linguistic Data Consortium.Google Scholar
  3. Chen, S. C. (2008). Neighborhood characteristics and phonetic-sound correspondence of Chinese characters. Chinese Journal of Psychology, 50, 223–239.Google Scholar
  4. Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204–256.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Fang, S. P., Horng, R. Y., & Tzeng, O. J. L. (1986). Consistency effects in the Chinese character and pseudo-character naming tests. In H. S. R. Kao & R. Hoosain (Eds.), Linguistics, psychology and the Chinese language (pp. 11–21). Hong Kong: University of Hong Kong Press.Google Scholar
  6. Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 35, 116–124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Glushko, R. J. (1979). The organization and activation of orthographic knowledge in reading aloud. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 674–691.Google Scholar
  8. Hue, C. W. (1992). Recognition processes in character naming. In H. C. Chen & O. J. L. Tzeng (Eds.), Language processing in Chinese (pp. 93–107). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Jared, D., McRae, K., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1990). The basis of consistency effects in word naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 687–715.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Jared, D. (1997). Evidence that strategy effects in word naming reflect changes in output timing rather than changes in processing route. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 1424–1438.Google Scholar
  11. Jared, D. (2002). Spelling-sound consistency and regularity effects in word naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 723–750.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Lee, C. Y., Tsai, J. L., Su, E. C. I., Tzeng, O. J. L., & Hung, D. L. (2005). Consistency, regularity and frequency effects in naming Chinese characters. Language and Linguistics, 6, 75–107.Google Scholar
  13. Lin, C. H., & Collins, P. (2012). The effects of L1 and orthographic regularity and consistency in naming Chinese characters. Reading and Writing, 25, 1747–1767.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Liu, I. M., Sue, I. R., & Chen, S. C. (2001). The phonetic function of Chinese phonetic radicals. Kaohsiung, Taiwan: Fu-Weng.Google Scholar
  15. Liu, Y., Hao, M., Shu, H., Tan, L. H., & Weekes, B. S. (2008). Age-of-acquisition effects on oral reading in Chinese. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 344–350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Lupker, S. J., Brown, P., & Colombo, L. (1997). Strategic control in a naming task: Changing routes or changing deadlines? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 570–590.Google Scholar
  17. Patterson, K., & Shewell, C. (1987). Speak and spell: Dissociations and word-class effects. In M. G. Coltheart, G. Sartori, & R. Job (Eds.), The cognitive neuropsychology of language (pp. 273–294). London: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  18. Perry, C., Ziegler, J. C., & Zorzi, M. (2007). Nested incremental modeling in the development of computational theories: The CDP+ model of reading aloud. Psychological Review, 114, 273–315.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Plaut, D. C., McClelland, J. L., Seidenberg, M. S., & Patterson, K. (1996). Understanding normal and impaired word reading: Computational principles in quasi-regular domains. Psychological Review, 103, 56–115.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Protopapas, A. (2007). CheckVocal: A program to facilitate checking the accuracy and response time of vocal responses from DMDX. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 39, 859–862.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Seidenberg, M. S., & McClelland, J. L. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96, 523–568.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Sue, I. R., & Liu, I. M. (2004). The phonetic representation function of phonetic radicals: Segmental and supra-segmental features. Chinese Journal of Psychology, 46, 231–248.Google Scholar
  23. Venezky, R. L. (1970). The structure of english orthography. NY Berlin: De Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Yang, J., McCandliss, B. D., Shu, H., & Zevin, J. D. (2009). Simulating language-specific and language-general effects in a statistical learning model of Chinese reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 61, 238–257.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Human DevelopmentTzu Chi UniversityHualienTaiwan
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyNational Cheng Kung UniversityTainan CityTaiwan

Personalised recommendations