Advertisement

Journal of Psycholinguistic Research

, Volume 42, Issue 1, pp 51–70 | Cite as

Escape from the Island: Grammaticality and (Reduced) Acceptability of wh-island Violations in Danish

  • Ken Ramshøj Christensen
  • Johannes Kizach
  • Anne Mette Nyvad
Article

Abstract

In the syntax literature, it is commonly assumed that a constraint on linguistic competence blocks extraction of wh-expressions (e.g. what or which book) from embedded questions, referred to as wh-islands. Furthermore, it is assumed that there is an argument/adjunct asymmetry in extraction from wh-islands. We report results from two acceptability judgment experiments on long and short wh-movement and wh-extraction from wh-islands in Danish. The results revealed four main findings: (1) No adjunct/argument asymmetry in extraction from wh-islands. (2) Long adjunct wh-movement is less acceptable than long argument wh-movement, and this difference is attributable to matrix verb compatibility and factivity, not D-linking. (3) Long movement reduces acceptability, but is more acceptable than island violations. (4) Training effects reveal that island violations, though degraded, are grammatical in Danish. Since the standard assumptions cannot account for the range of results, we argue in favor of a processing account referring to locality (processing domains) and working memory.

Keywords

Parsing Working memory Training effects wh-movement D-linking 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Bader M., Meng M. (1999) Subject-object ambiguities in German embedded clauses: An across-the-board comparison. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 28: 121–143CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bastiaanse R. J., Bouma G., Post W. (2009) Linguistic complexity and frequency in agrammatic speech production. Brain and Language 109: 18–28PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Branigan H. (2007) Syntactic priming. Language and Linguistics Compass 1.1-2: 1–16CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Caplan D., Waters G. S. (1999) Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22: 77–126PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Chomsky N. A. (1973) Conditions on transformations. In: Anderson S., Kiparsky P. (eds) A festschrift for Morris Halle. Holt, Rinehart, pp 232–286Google Scholar
  6. Chomsky N. A. (1986) Barriers. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  7. Chomsky N. A. (1995) The minimalist program. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  8. Chomsky N. A. (2001) Derivation by phase. In: Kenstowicz M. (Ed.), Ken Hale. A life in language. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 1–52Google Scholar
  9. Chomsky N. A. (2005) Three factors in language design. Linguistic Inquiry 36.1: 1–22CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Christensen K. R., Wallentin M. (2011) The locative alternation: Distinguishing linguistic processing cost from error signals in Broca’s region. NeuroImage 56.3: 1622–1631CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Clifton C. Jr., Frazier L. (1989) Comprehending sentences with long-distance dependencies. In: Carlson G. N., Tannenhaus M. K. (eds) Linguistic structure in language processing. Kluwer, Dordrecht, pp 273–317CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Clifton C. Jr., Frazier L. (2004) Should given information come before new? Yes and no. Memory & Cognition 32.6: 886–895CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Clifton C. Jr., Fanselow G., Frazier L. (2006) Amnestying superiority violations: Processing multiple questions. Linguistic Inquiry 37.1: 51–68CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Fanselow G., Frisch S. (2006) Effects of processing difficulty on judgments of acceptability. In: Fanselow G., Fery C., Schlesewsky M. (eds) Gradience in grammar: Generative perspectives. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 291–316CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Fanselow G., Kliegl R., Schlesewsky M. (1999) Processing difficulty and principles of grammar. In: Kemper S., Kliegl R. (eds) Constraints on language: Aging, grammar and memory. Kluwer, Dortrecht, pp 171–202Google Scholar
  16. Featherston S. (2005) Universals and grammaticality: Wh-constraints in German and English. Linguistics 43.4: 667–711CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Ferreira F., Christianson K., Hollingworth A. (2001) Misinterpretations of garden-path sentences: Implications for models of reanalysis. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 30: 3–20PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Fodor J. D., Inoue A. (1998) Attach anyway. In: Fodor J. D., Ferreira F. (eds) Reanalysis in sentence processing. Kluwer Academic Publishers, Dordrecht, pp 101–141CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Frazier L., Clifton C. Jr. (2002) Processing “d-linked” phrases. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 31.6: 633–659CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Gibson E. (1998) Linguistic complexity: Locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition 68: 1–76PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Gibson E. (2000) The dependency locality theory: A distance-based theory of linguistic complexity. In: Miyashita Y., Marantz A., O’Neil W. (eds) Image, language, brain. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 95–126Google Scholar
  22. Grodzinsky Y. (1995) A restrictive theory on agrammatic comprehension. Brain and Language 50: 27–51PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Grodzinsky Y. (2000) The neurology of syntax: Language use without Broca’s area. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23: 1–71PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hakuri T. E., Levine R. D. (1995) Adjunct extraction. Journal of Linguistics 31.2: 195–226CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hawkins J. A. (1994) A performance theory of order and constituency. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  26. Hawkins J. A. (2004) Efficiency and complexity in grammars. Oxford University Press, OxfordCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hofmeister P., Sag I. (2010) Cognitive constraints and island effects. Language 86(2): 366–415PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Kiparsky P., Kiparsky C. (1970) Fact. In: Bierwisch M., Heidolph K. (eds) Progress in linguistics. Mouton, The Hague, pp 143–173Google Scholar
  29. Kluender R. (1998) On the distinction between strong and weak islands: A processing perspective. In: Culicover P., McNally L. (eds) Syntax and semantics, vol. 29: The limits of syntax. Academic Press, San Diego, CA, pp 241–279Google Scholar
  30. Kluender R., Kutas M. (1993) Subjacency as a processing phenomenon. Language and Cognitive Processes 8: 573–633CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Luka B. J., Barsalou L. W. (2005) Structural facilitation: Mere exposure effects for grammatical acceptability as evidence for syntactic priming in comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language 52: 436–459CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Pesetsky D. (1987) Wh-in situ and unselective binding. In: Reuland E., Meulen A. (eds) The representation of (in-) definiteness. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 98–129Google Scholar
  33. Phillips C., Kazanina N., Abada S. H. (2005) ERP effects of the processing of syntactic long-distance dependencies. Cognitive Brain Research 22.3: 407–428CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Phillips, C. (forthcoming). Some arguments and nonarguments for reductionist accounts of syntactic phenomena. Language and Cognitive Processes. doi: 10.1080/01690965.2010.530960.
  35. Pickering M., Barton S., Shillcock R. (1994) Unbounded dependencies, island constraints, and processing complexity. In: Clifton C., Frazier L., Rayner K. (eds) Perspectives on sentence processing. Erlbaum, London, pp 199–224Google Scholar
  36. Pickering M., Traxler M. (2001) Strategies for processing unbounded dependencies: Lexical information and verb-argument assignment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 27: 1401–1410PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Poole G. (2011) Syntactic theory (2nd ed.). Palgrave MacMillan, HoundmillsGoogle Scholar
  38. Poulsen M. (2008) Acceptability and processing of long-distance dependencies in Danish. Nordic Journal of Linguistics 31.1: 73–107Google Scholar
  39. Pritchett, B. L. (1992). Grammatical competence and parsing performance. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  40. Rizzi L. (1990) Relativized minimality. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  41. Rizzi L. (2004) Locality and left periphery. In: Belletti A. (Ed.), Structures and beyond. The cartography of syntactic structures (Vol. 3). Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 223–251Google Scholar
  42. Roberts R., Gibson E. (2002) Individual differences in sentence memory. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 31.6: 573–598CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Ross, J. R. (1967). Constraints on variables in syntax. PhD dissertation, MIT, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  44. Sabel J. (2002) A minimalist analysis of syntactic islands. The Linguistic Review 19: 271–315CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Snyder W. (2000) An experimental investigation of syntactic satiation effects. Linguistic Inquiry 31: 575–582CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Sprouse J. (2007) Continuous acceptability, categorical grammaticality, and experimental syntax. Biolinguistics 1: 123–134Google Scholar
  47. Sprouse J. (2008) The differential sensitivity of acceptability judgments to processing effects. Linguistic Inquiry 39.4: 686–694CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Sprouse, J., Wagers, M., & Phillips, C. (2012). A test of the relation between working memory capacity and syntactic islands effects. Language(to appear) (pre-print version available online at: http://www.socsci.uci.edu/~jsprouse/).
  49. Vikner S. (1995) Verb movement and expletive subjects in the Germanic languages. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  50. Warren T., Gibson E. (2002) The influence of referential processing on sentence complexity. Cognition 85: 79–112PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  • Ken Ramshøj Christensen
    • 1
    • 2
  • Johannes Kizach
    • 2
  • Anne Mette Nyvad
    • 2
  1. 1.Center for Functionally Integrative NeuroscienceAarhus University HospitalAarhus CDenmark
  2. 2.Department of Aesthetics and CommunicationAarhus UniversityAarhus CDenmark

Personalised recommendations