Advertisement

Journal of Nonverbal Behavior

, Volume 34, Issue 1, pp 43–56 | Cite as

Behavioral Stability Across Time and Situations: Nonverbal Versus Verbal Consistency

  • Max Weisbuch
  • Michael L. Slepian
  • Asha Clarke
  • Nalini Ambady
  • Jeremy Veenstra-VanderWeele
Original Paper

Abstract

Behavioral consistency has been at the center of debates regarding the stability of personality. We argue that people are consistent but that such consistency is best observed in nonverbal behavior. In Study 1, participants’ verbal and nonverbal behaviors were observed in a mock interview and then in an informal interaction. In Study 2, medical students’ verbal and nonverbal behaviors were observed during first- and third-year clinical skills evaluation. Nonverbal behavior exhibited consistency across context and time (a duration of 2 years) whereas verbal behavior did not. Discussion focuses on implications for theories of personality and nonverbal behavior.

Keywords

Consistency Nonverbal behavior Individual differences 

Notes

Acknowledgments

The project described was supported by NIMH grant F32MH078350 to the first author and by NIH grant R01 MH70833 to the fourth author. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute of Mental Health or the National Institutes of Health.

References

  1. Ambady, N., Bernieri, F. J., & Richeson, J. A. (2000). Toward a histology of social behavior: Judgmental accuracy from thin slices of the behavioral stream. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, 32 (pp. 201–272). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  2. Ambady, N., LaPlante, D., Nguyen, T., Rosenthal, R., Chaumeton, N., & Levinson, W. (2002). Surgeons’ tone of voice: A clue to malpractice history. Surgery, 132, 5–9.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1992). Thin slices of expressive behavior as predictors of interpersonal consequences: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 256–274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1993). Half a minute: Predicting teacher evaluations from thin slices of nonverbal behavior and physical attractiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 431–441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bem, D. J., & Allen, A. (1974). Predicting some of the people some of the time: The search for cross-situational consistencies in behavior. Psychological Review, 81, 506–520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Blanck, P. D., Rosenthal, R., & Cordell, L. D. H. (1985). The appearance of justice: Judges verbal and nonverbal behaviour in criminal jury trials. Stanford Law Review, 38, 89–164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Borkenau, P., Mauer, N., Riemann, R., Spinath, F. M., & Angleitner, A. (2004). Thin slices of behavior as cues of personality and intelligence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 599–614.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Cervone, D., & Shoda, Y. (1999). Beyond traits in the study of personality coherence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8, 27–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1988). Personality in adulthood: A six-year longitudinal study of self-reports and spouse ratings on the NEO personality inventory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 853–863.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. DePaulo, B. M. (1992). Nonverbal behavior and self-presentation. Psychological Review, 111, 203–243.Google Scholar
  11. Diener, E., & Larsen, R. J. (1984). Temporal stability and cross-situational consistency of affective, cognitive, and behavioral responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 871–883.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Ekman, P., Davidson, R. J., & Friesen, W. V. (1990). Duchenne’s smile: Emotional expression and brain physiology II. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 342–353.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). Nonverbal leakage and cues to deception. Psychiatry, 32, 88–106.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1974). Detecting deception from the body or face. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29, 288–289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Endler, N. S. (1973). The person versus the situation: A pseudo issue? A response to Alker. Journal of Personality, 41, 287–303.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Epstein, S. (1979). The stability of behavior: I. On predicting most of the people much of the time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1097–1126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Fleeson, W. (2001). Toward a structure- and process-integrated view of personality: Traits as density distributions of states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 1011–1027.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Fleeson, W. (2007). Situation-based contingencies underlying trait-content manifestation in behavior. Journal of Personality, 75, 825–862.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Funder, D. C. (2006). Towards a resolution of the personality triad: Persons, situations, and behaviors. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 21–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Funder, D. C., & Colvin, C. R. (1991). Explorations in behavioral consistency: Properties of persons, situations, and behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 773–794.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Furr, R. M., & Funder, D. C. (2004). Situational similarity and behavioral consistency: Subjective, objective, variable-centered, and person-centered approaches. Journal of Research in Personality, 38, 421–447.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hager, J. C., & Ekman, P. (1985). The asymmetry of facial actions is inconsistent with models of hemispheric specialization. Psychophysiology, 22, 307–318.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Hartshorne, H., & May, A. (1928). Studies in the nature of character, vol. 1, studies in deceit. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  24. Keltner, D., & Buswell, B. N. (1997). Embarrassment: Its distinct form and appeasement functions. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 250–270.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Kenrick, D. T., & Funder, D. C. (1988). Profiting from controversy: Lessons from the person-situation debate. American Psychologist, 43, 23–34.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Lang, P. J., Davis, M., & Ohman, A. (2000). Fear and anxiety: Animal models and human cognitive psychophysiology. Journal of Affective Disorders, 61, 137–159.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Mischel, W. (1968). Personality and assessment. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  28. Mischel, W., & Peake, P. K. (1982). Beyond déjà vu in the search for cross-situational consistency. Psychological Review, 89, 730–755.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1995). A cognitive-affective system theory of personality: Reconceptualizing situations, dispositions, dynamics, and invariance in personality structure. Psychological Review, 102, 246–268.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1998). Reconciling processing dynamics and personality dispositions. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 229–258.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Mendoza-Denton, R. (2002). Situation—behavior profiles as a locus of consistency in personality. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 50–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Moskowitz, D. S. (1982). Coherence and cross-situational generality in personality: A new analysis of old problems. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 754–768.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Niedenthal, P. M. (2007). Embodying emotion. Science, 316, 1002–1005.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Pinker, S. (1991). Rules of language. Science, 253, 530–535.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 10, pp. 173–220). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Scherer, K. R. (2005). Unconscious processes in emotion: The bulk of the iceberg. In L. F. Barrett, P. M. Niedenthal, & P. Winkielman (Eds.), Emotion and consciousness (pp. 312–334). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  37. Shoda, Y., & Mischel, W. (2000). Reconciling contextualism with the core assumptions of personality psychology. European Journal of Personality, 14, 407–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Streeter, L. A., Krauss, R. M., Geller, V. J., Olson, C. T., & Apple, W. (1977). Pitch changes during attempted deception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 345–350.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Ten Berge, M. A., & De Raad, B. (2002). The structure of situations from a personality perspective. European Journal of Personality, 16, 81–102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Tickle-Degnen, L., & Puccinelli, N. M. (1999). The nonverbal expression of negative emotions: Peer and supervisor responses to occupational therapy students’ emotional attributes. Occupational Therapy Journal of Research, 19, 18–29.Google Scholar
  41. Vansteelandt, K., & Van Mechelen, I. V. (2004). The personality triad in balance: Multidimensional individual differences in situation-behavior profiles. Journal of Research in Personality, 38, 367–393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Walker-Andrews, A. S. (2008). Intermodal emotional processes in infancy. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland, & L. Feldman-Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 364–375). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  43. Zuckerman, M., DePaulo, B. M., & Rosenthal, R. (1981). Verbal and nonverbal communication of deception. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 14, pp. 1–59). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  • Max Weisbuch
    • 1
  • Michael L. Slepian
    • 1
  • Asha Clarke
    • 1
  • Nalini Ambady
    • 1
  • Jeremy Veenstra-VanderWeele
    • 2
  1. 1.Psychology DepartmentTufts UniversityMedfordUSA
  2. 2.Department of PsychiatryVanderbilt Medical Center, Vanderbilt UniversityNashvilleUSA

Personalised recommendations