Skip to main content
Log in

Attribution of Emotions to Story Characters by Severely and Profoundly Deaf Children

  • Published:
Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Simple stories were presented to severely or profoundly prelingually deafened children and hearing controls in two age groups. Participants were asked to choose the appropriate emotion for the central characters. While at either age level, the hearing children's success rate was significantly higher than that of the deaf children, the older deaf children showed a marked improvement compared with the younger deaf children. In each age group, the profiles of the average scores of the deaf children over the six emotions studied were similar to those of their hearing peers, suggesting that the deaf children found the same emotions easier (or more difficult) to assign to the story characters. It would appear that deaf and hearing children code their emotional experiences using the same categories, which are progressively refined with age. The developmental delay in deaf children's emotional understanding is interpreted in terms of the social experience of the deaf child of hearing parents, which affords limited opportunity to discuss emotional causality.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • British Association of Teachers of the Deaf (1981). Audiological definitions and forms for recording audiometric information. J. Brit. Assoc. Teach. Deaf 5: 83–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brody, L., and Harrison, R. (1987). Developmental changes in children's abilities to match and label emotionally laden situations. Motiv. Emotion 11: 347–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bullock, M., and Russell, J. A. (1984). Preschool children's interpretation of facial expressions of emotion. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 7: 193–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, C., Felleman, E., and Masters, J. (1983). Influence of children's emotional states on the recognition of emotion in peers and social motives to change another's emotional state. Motiv. Emotion 7: 61–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke School for the Deaf (1953). 86th Annual Report, Northampton, MA.

  • DeConti, K. A., and Dickerson, D. J. (1994). Preschool childrens understanding of the situational determinants of others emotions. Cogn. Emotion 8: 453–472.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denham, S. (1986a). Affective understanding in young preschoolers and reactions to peers’ emotions. Paper presented at the American Psychological Association, Washington, DC.

  • Denham, S. A. (1986b). Social cognition, prosocial behaviour, and emotion in preschoolers: contextual validation. Child Dev. 57: 194–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denham, S. A., and Couchoud, E. A. (1990). Young preschoolers’ understanding of emotion. Child Study J. 20: 171–192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dorr, A. (1985). Contexts for experience with emotion, with special attention to television. In Lewis, M. and Saarni, C. (eds.), The Socialization of Emotion, Plenum Press, New York, pp. 55–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, J., Bretherton, I., and Munn, P. (1987). Conversations about feeling states between mothers and their young children. Dev. Psychol. 23: 132–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, J., Brown, J., and Beardsall, L. (1991). Family talk about feeling states and children's later understanding of others’ emotions. Dev. Psychol. 27: 448–455.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, J., and Brown, J. R. (1993). Early conversations about causality: content, pragmatics and developmental change. Brit. J. Dev. Psychol.

  • Fabes, R. A., Eisenberg, N., Nyman, M., and Michealieu, Q. (1991). Young children's appraisal of others’ spontaneous emotional reactions. Dev. Psychol. 27: 858–866.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, B. J., and Exline, R. V. (1987). Social visual interaction: A conceptual and literature review. In Siegman, A. W., and Feldstein, S. (eds.), Nonverbal Behavior and Communication, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, New Jersey, pp. 225–326.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, C., Banks, J., Fyfe, R., and Morris, A. (1992). The use of verbatim and schematic strategies in the recall of written stories by deaf and hearing children. Brit. J. Educ. Psychol. 62: 88–105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, C.D., Hosie, J.A., Russell, P., and Ormel, E. (2001). Emotional development in deaf children: Understanding facial expressions, display rules and theory of mind. In Marschark, M., Clark, D., and Karchmer, M. (eds.), Context, Cognition and Deafness, Gallaudet University Press, Washington, pp. 135–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gregory, S. (1976). The Deaf Child and His Family, Halsted Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hadwin, J., and Perner, J. (1991). Pleased and surprised: Children's cognitive theory of emotion. Brit. J. Dev. Psychol. 9: 215–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, D. B. (1963). Children's Drawings as Measures of Intellectual Maturity, Harcourt, Brace and World, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, P. L. (1985). What children know about the situations that provoke emotion. In Lewis, M., and Saarni, C. (eds.), The Socialization of Emotion, Plenum Press, New York, pp. 161–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Higgenbotham, D. J., and Baker, B. M. (1981). Social participation and cognitive play differences in hearing-impaired and normally hearing preschoolers. Volta Rev. 83: 135–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hosie, J. A., Gray, C. D., Russell, P. A., Scott, C., and Hunter, N. (1998). The matching of facial expressions by deaf and hearing children and their production and comprehension of emotion labels. Motiv. Emotion 22: 293–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hosie, J. A., Russell, P. A., Gray, C. D., Scott, C., Hunter, N., Banks, J. S., and Macaulay, M. C. (2000). Knowledge of display rules in prelingually deaf and hearing children. J. Child Psychol. Psychiat. 41: 389–398.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ireson, J., and Shields, M. (1982). The development of the young child's representation of emotion (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 263 983). London, England: University of London Institute of Education.

  • Lederberg, A. R., and Mobley, C. E. (1990). The effect of hearing impairment on the quality of attachment and mother-toddler interaction. Child Dev. 61: 1596–1604.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Markham, R., and Adams, K. (1992). The effect of type of task on children's identification of facial expressions. J. Nonverbal Behav. 16: 21–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michalson, L., and Lewis, M. (1985). What do children know about emotions and when do they know it? In Lewis, M., and Saarni, C. (eds.), The Socialization of Emotions, Plenum, New York, pp. 117–139.

    Google Scholar 

  • Odom, P. B., Blanton, R. L., and Laukhuf, C. (1973). Facial expressions and interpretation of emotion-arousing situations in deaf and hearing children. J. Abnormal Child Psychol. 1: 139–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C. C., and Siegal, M. (1995). Deafness, conversation and theory of mind. J. Child Psychol. Psychiat. 36: 459–474.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C. C., and Siegal, M. (1997). Psychological, biological and physical thinking in normal, autistic and deaf children. In Wellman, H. M., and Inagaki, K. (eds.), The Emergence of Core Domains of Thought, Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, pp. 55–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reichenbach, L., and Masters, J. C. (1983). Childrens use of expressive and contextual cues in judgements of emotion. Child Dev. 54: 993–1004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Remmel, E., Bettger, J. G., and Weinberg, A. M. (2001). Theory of mind development in deaf children. In Clark, M. D., Marschark, M., and Karchmer, M. (eds.), Context, Cognition and Deafness, Gallaudet University Press, Washington, DC, pp. 113–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rieffe, C., and Meerum Terwogt, M. (2000). Deaf children's understanding of emotions: Desires take preference. J. Child Psychol. Psychiat. 41: 601–608.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 39: 1161–1178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. A., and Bullock, M. (1985). Multidimensional scaling of emotional facial expressions: Similarity from preschoolers to adults. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 48: 1290–1298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. A., and Bullock, M. (1986). On the dimensions preschoolers use to interpret facial expressions of emotion. Dev. Psychol. 22: 97–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. A., Suzuki, N., and Ishida, N. (1993). Canadian, Greek and Japanese freely produced emotion labels for facial expressions. Motiv. Emotion 17: 337–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spelke, E. S., and Cortelyou, A. (1981). Perceptual aspects of social knowing: Looking and listening in infancy. In Lamb, M. E., and Sherrod, L. R. (eds.), Infant Social Cognition: Empirical and Theoretical Considerations, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 61–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stein, N. (1979). How children understand stories: A developmental analysis. In Katz, L. G. (ed.), Current Topics in Early Childhood, Ablex, Norwood, N. J., Vol. 2, pp. 261–290.

  • Stein, N., and Glenn, C. (1979). An analysis of story comprehension in elementary school children. In Freedle, R. (ed.), Advances in Discourse Processes: New directions in Discourse Processing, Ablex, Norwood, N. J., Vol. 2, pp. 261–190.

  • Vandell, D., and George, L. (1981). Social interaction in hearing and deaf preschoolers: Successes and failures in initiation. Child Dev. 52: 627–635.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Walden, T. A. (1991). Infant social referencing. In Garber, J., and Dodge, K. A. (eds.), The Development of Emotion Regulation and Dysregulation. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 69–88.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wedell-Monnig, J., and Lumley, J. M. (1980). Child deafness and mother-child interaction. Child Dev. 51: 766–774.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., Harris, P. L., Banerjee, M., and Sinclair, A. (1995). Early understanding of emotion: Evidence from natural language. Cognition Emotion 2(3): 117–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wertheimer, M. (1961). Psychomotor coordination of auditory and visual space at birth. Science 134: 1692.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank Mairi Macaulay (Head Teacher), Margaret Falconer, Joan Grant, and the other teachers at the Aberdeen School for the Deaf for their help at all stages of the research and for their unfailing support and encouragement. We would also like to acknowledge the support of the Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC: Grant Number R000236272).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Colin Gray.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Gray, C., Hosie, J., Russell, P. et al. Attribution of Emotions to Story Characters by Severely and Profoundly Deaf Children. J Dev Phys Disabil 19, 145–159 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10882-006-9029-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10882-006-9029-1

Key Words

Navigation