Skip to main content
Log in

The productivity of variable disyllabic tone sandhi in Tianjin Chinese

  • Published:
Journal of East Asian Linguistics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Tianjin Chinese has one of the more complex tone sandhi systems in Northern Chinese dialects. Due to its close contact with Standard Chinese, many of its tone sandhi patterns are also variable. This article first reports a detailed acoustic study of tone sandhi patterns in both real lexical items and novel words in Tianjin. The data were collected from 48 speakers of Tianjin, who were instructed to pronounce disyllabic sequences as real words based on voice prompts. The results showed that the productivity of the sandhis in novel words varied depending on the sandhi—some were less productive than in real words, and some were more productive, indicating a combination of underlearning, overlearning, and proper learning of the sandhis from the lexicon. A theoretical model that predicts the productivity patterns based on the phonetic properties of the sandhis and statistical generalizations about the sandhis over the lexicon is then proposed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The transcriptions of the Tianjin tones vary from source to source. For example, using Chao’s tone numbers (Chao 1968), Li and Liu (1985) transcribed the four tones as 21, 45, 213, 54, respectively, while Shi (1990) used 11, 55, 24, 53. We use Chen’s (2000) notation here. For more detailed discussion and acoustic data on Tianjin citation tones, see Zhang and Liu (2011).

  2. In addition, Shi and Wang (2004) also found that for T1+T1, younger speakers (<20 years) consistently used T2+T1 as the sandhi tones, not the previously reported T3+T1, while older speakers (>70%) varied between T3+T1 and T2+T1. See Lu (1997, 2004) and Zhang and Liu (2011) for similar findings and additional discussions.

  3. An anonymous reviewer questioned the phonetic basis of the T3+T2 sandhi as the opposite pattern, whereby L+H → LH+H, is attested in African languages. But this type of regressive tone spreading is considerably rarer than progressive assimilation (Maddieson 1978; Hyman 2007; Zhang 2007). Hyman (2007) in fact goes on to argue that regressive tone spreading is due to special circumstances involving tone attraction to stressed positions or pressure from intonation at the right edge and therefore is not a diachronically natural process.

  4. Although neither author is a native speaker of Tianjin, we believe that our judgment was accurate as stressless syllables in Tianjin have significantly reduced duration (Jiang 1994), similar to Standard Chinese.

  5. A reviewer asked what restrictions UseListed constraints would have and whether they only refer to tones of a given language. These are interesting and difficult questions. Provided that (a) a lexical phonological pattern is not entirely productive, and (b) there are productivity differences among different types of phonological patterns, indicating that the lack of full productivity is not just a task effect, it is necessary to encode the effect of lexicality for this pattern in the grammar. Therefore, UseListed constraints would be applicable to any type of phonological pattern, not just tonal ones. It is possible to conceive of UseListed constraints simply as IO-faithfulness constraints, which would require the output to be identical to the listed form. This is essentially how we have used these constraints here. It is then less of a surprise that these constraints are applicable to other phonological features. In a published update of Zuraw (2000), Zuraw (2010) in fact rephrased the UseListed constraints in similar terms and distinguished the correspondence between the output and the listed form and the correspondence between the output and the “underlying” form by shifting the burden of the latter to Output–Output-correspondence. We have simply maintained the distinction between UseListed and IO-faithfulness here. The proliferation of the UseListed constraints is necessary for the analysis of lexical frequency effects on productivity as well as lexical variation, and Coetzee (2009), Becker et al. (2011), and Coetzee and Kawahara (2013), among others, have used a similar strategy.

  6. The markedness constraints should be taken as phonotactic generalizations that speakers make when tonal alternations are encountered. This is different from the canonical OT assumption that all constraints are in UG (Prince and Smolensky 1993). For modeling the learning of phonotactic constraints, see Hayes and Wilson (2008).

  7. The reason we use Preserve instead of Ident in our faithfulness constraints is that in its formal definition, Ident(F) requires [F] to be a distinctive feature; the featural representation of tone, however, is controversial in both the number of tone levels and whether there are contour tone features (see Zhang 2010 for a review of the issue). We have therefore chosen to use the theory-neutral Preserve to avoid this controversy.

References

  • Albright, Adam. 2002. Islands of reliability for regular morphology: Evidence from Italian. Language 78(4): 684–709.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Albright, Adam, and Bruce Hayes. 2003. Rules vs. analogy in English past tenses: A computational/experimental study. Cognition 90: 119–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Albright, Adam, Argelia Andrade, and Bruce Hayes. 2001. Segmental environments of Spanish diphthongization. In UCLA working papers in linguistics 7, (Papers in phonology 5), ed. Adam Albright, and Taehong Cho, 117–151. Los Angeles: UCLA Department of Linguistics.

  • Becker, Michael, Nihan Ketrez, and Andrew Nevins. 2011. The surfeit of the stimulus: Analytic biases filter lexical statistics in Turkish laryngeal alternations. Language 87: 84–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berent, Iris, Donca Steriade, Tracy Lennertz, and Vered Vaknin. 2007. What we know about what we have never heard: Evidence from perceptual illusions. Cognition 104: 591–630.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berko, Jean. 1958. The child’s learning of English morphology. Word 14: 150–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boersma, Paul and David Weenink. 2009. Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (computer program). http://www.praat.org/. Accessed 5 Jan 2009.

  • Bybee, Joan. 2001. Phonology and language use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bybee, Joan. 2006. From usage to grammar: The mind’s response to repetition. Language 82: 711–733.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bybee, Joan, and Elly Pardo. 1981. On lexical and morphological conditioning of alternations: A nonce-probe experiment with Spanish verbs. Linguistics 19: 937–968.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chao, Yuen Ren. 1968. A grammar of spoken Chinese. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, Matthew Y. 1987. The syntax of Xiamen tone sandhi. Phonology Yearbook 4: 109–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, Matthew Y. 2000. Tone sandhi: Patterns across Chinese dialects. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Cheng, Robert L. 1968. Tone sandhi in Taiwanese. Linguistics 41: 19–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coetzee, Andries W. 2009. Learning lexical indexation. Phonology 26: 109–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coetzee, Andries W., and Shigeto Kawahara. 2013. Frequency biases in phonological variation. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 31: 47–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coetzee, Andries W., and Joe Pater. 2008. Weighted constraints and gradient restrictions on place co-occurrence in Muna and Arabic. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 26: 289–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coetzee, Andries, and Joe Pater. 2011. The place of variation in phonological theory. In The handbook of phonological theory, 2nd ed, ed. John A. Goldsmith, Jason Riggle, and Alan C.L. Yu, 401–434. Cambridge, MA and Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Da, Jun. 2004. Chinese text computing. http://lingua.mtsu.edu/chinese-computing. Accessed 1 Sept 2008.

  • Gahl, Susanne, and Alan Yu. 2006. Special issue on exemplar-based models in linguistics. The Linguistic Review 23(3): 289–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gandour, Jackson T., Siripong Potisuk, and Sumalee Dechongkit. 1994. Tonal coarticulation in Thai. Journal of Phonetics 22: 474–492.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gao, Jing. 2004. The changing sandhi rules in Tianjin dialect. In Phonetic and phonological studies on Tianjin dialect, ed. Lu Jilun, 193–247. Beijing: Beijing Institute of Technology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldwater, Sharon, and Mark Johnson. 2003. Learning OT constraint ranking using a maximum entropy model. In Proceedings of the Stockholm workshop on variation within optimality theory, ed. Jennifer Spenader, Anders Eriksson, and Osten Dahl, 111–120. Stockholm: Stockholm University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, Bruce, and Zsuzsa C. Londe. 2006. Stochastic phonological knowledge: The case of Hungarian vowel harmony. Phonology 23: 59–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, Bruce, and Colin Wilson. 2008. A maximum entropy model of phonotactics and phonotactic learning. Linguistic Inquiry 39: 379–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, Bruce, Colin Wilson, and Benjamin George. 2009a. Maxent grammar tool. Java program. http://www.linguistics.ucla.edu/people/hayes/MaxentGrammarTool/. Accessed 22 May 2009.

  • Hayes, Bruce, Kie Zuraw, Péter Siptár, and Zsuzsa Londe. 2009b. Natural and unnatural constraints in Hungarian vowel harmony. Language 85: 822–863.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herd, Wendy, Allard Jongman, and Joan Sereno. 2010. An acoustic and perceptual analysis of /t/ and /d/ flaps in American English. Journal of Phonetics 38: 504–516.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hsieh, Hsin-I. 1970. The psychological reality of tone sandhi rules in Taiwanese. In Papers from the 6th meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, ed. M.A. Campbell, 489–503. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hsieh, Hsin-I. 1975. How generative is phonology. In The transformational-generative paradigm and modern linguistic theory, ed. E.F. Koerner, 109–144. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hsieh, Hsin-I. 1976. On the unreality of some phonological rules. Lingua 38: 1–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hyman, Larry. 2007. Universals of tone rules: 30 years later. In Tones and tunes vol 1: Typological studies in word and sentence prosody, ed. Tomas Riad, and Carlos Gussenhoven, 1–34. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Jäger, Gerhard. 2007. Maximum Entropy models and stochastic optimality theory. In Architectures, rules and preferences: Variation on themes by Joan W. Bresnan, ed. Annie Zaenen, Jane Simpson, Tracy H. King, Jane Grimshaw, Joan Maling, and Chris Manning, 467–479. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jassem, Wiktor, and Lutoslawa Richter. 1989. Neutralization of voicing in Polish obstruents. Journal of Phonetics 17: 317–325.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jiang, Hui. 1994. The phonetic description of neutral tone in Tianjin dialect. MA thesis, Tianjin Normal University, Tianjin.

  • Kenstowicz, Michael, and Charles Kisseberth. 1979. Generative phonology: Description and theory. San Diego: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiparsky, Paul. 1973. Abstractness, opacity, and global rules. In Three dimensions of linguistic theory, ed. Osamu Fujimura, 57–86. Tokyo: TEC Company Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirchner, Robert. 1996. Synchronic chain shifts in optimality theory. Linguistic Inquiry 27: 341–350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leben, William. 1973. Suprasegmental phonology. PhD dissertation, MIT.

  • Li, Xing-Jian, and Si-Xun Liu. 1985. Tianjin fangyan de liandu biandiao [Tone sandhi in the Tianjin dialect]. Zhongguo Yuwen [Studies of the Chinese Language] 1985(1): 76–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liang, Yuzhang, and Aizhen Feng. 1996. Fuzhouhua yindang [The sound system of Fuzhou dialect]. Shanghai: Shanghai Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lin, Huishan. 2008. Variable directional applications in Tianjin tone sandhi. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 17: 181–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, Yu-Zhen, and Jiang Gao. 2003. Qu-Qu liandu biandiao guize: shehui yuyanxue bianxiang [FF sandhi rule in Tianjin dialect: A sociolinguistic variable]. Tianjin Shifan Daxue Xuebao—Shehui Kexue Ban [Journal of Tianjin Normal University—Social Sciences] 2003(5): 65–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lu, Ji-Lun. 1997. Tianjin fangyan zhong de yizhong xin de liandu biandiao [A new tone sandhi rule in Tianjin dialect]. Tianjin Shida Xuebao [Journal of Tianjin Normal University] 1997(4): 67–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lu, Ji-Lun. 2004. A new phenomenon in Tianjin tone sandhi. In Phonetic and phonological studies on Tianjin dialect: Festschrift for Professor Wang Jialing’s 70th birthday, ed. Lu Ji-Lun, 89–137. Beijing: Beijing Institute of Technology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ma, Qiuwu, and Yuan Jia. 2006. Tianjinhua shangsheng de liangtiao “biandiao guize” bianxi [Two new third tone sandhi rules in Tianjin dialect—a critical reanalysis]. Tianjin Shifan Daxue Xuebao—Shehui Kexue Ban [Journal of Tianjin Normal University—Social Science] 2006(1): 53–58.

  • Maddieson, Ian. 1978. Universals of tone. In Universals of human language, vol. 2: Phonology, ed. Joseph H. Greenberg, 335–366. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moreton, Elliott. 2004. Non-computable functions in optimality theory. In Optimality theory in phonology, ed. John McCarthy, 141–164. Malden: Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Moreton, Elliott. 2008. Analytical bias and phonological typology. Phonology 25: 83–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peng, Shu-Hui. 1997. Production and perception of Taiwanese tones in different tonal and prosodic contexts. Journal of Phonetics 25: 371–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peng, Shu-Hui. 2000. Lexical versus ‘phonological’ representations of Mandarin sandhi tones. In Language acquisition and the lexicon: Papers in laboratory phonology 5, ed. Michael B. Broe, and Janet B. Pierrehumbert, 152–167. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pierrehumbert, Janet B. 2001. Exemplar dynamics: Word frequency, lenition and contrast. In Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure, ed. Joan Bybee, and Paul Hopper, 137–157. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Pierrehumbert, Janet B. 2002. Word-specific phonetics. In Laboratory phonology 7, ed. Carlos Gussenhoven, and Natasha Warner, 101–139. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pierrehumbert, Janet B. 2006. The statistical basis of an unnatural alternation. In Laboratory phonology 8. Varieties of phonological competence, ed. Louis Goldstein, Douglas H. Whalen, and Catherine Best, 81–107. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

  • Port, Robert, and Penny Crawford. 1989. Incomplete neutralization and pragmatics in German. Journal of Phonetics 17: 257–282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prince, Alan, and Paul Smolensky. 1993. Optimality theory: Constraint interactions in generative grammar. New Brunswick: Rutgers Center for Cognitive Science, Rutgers University. (re-printed in 2004 by MIT Press, Cambridge, MA).

    Google Scholar 

  • Rietveld, Toni, and Aoju Chen. 2006. How to obtain and process perceptual judgements of intonational meaning. In Methods in empirical prosody research, ed. Stefan Sudhoff, Denisa Lenortová, Roland Meyer, Sandra Pappert, Petra Augurzky, Ina Mleinek, Nicole Richter, and Johannes Schieβer, 283–319. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rose, Phil. 1987. Considerations in the normalization of the fundamental frequency in linguistic tone. Speech Communication 6: 343–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shi, Feng. 1986. Tianjin fangyan shuangzizu shengdiao fenxi [An analysis of disyllabic tones in Tianjin dialect]. Yuyan Yanjiu [Linguistic Research] 1986(1): 77–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shi, Feng. 1988. Shilun Tianjinhua de shengdiao jiqi bianhua—xiandai yuyinxue biji [On tones and their recent changes in Tianjin dialect—modern phonetics notes]. Zhongguo Yuwen [Studies of the Chinese Language] 1988(5): 351–360.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shi, Feng. 1990. Hanyu he Dong-Tai yu de shengdiao geju [Tone systems in Chinese and Kam-Tai languages]. PhD dissertation, Nankai University, Tianjin.

  • Shi, Feng, and Ping Wang. 2004. Tianjinhua shengdiao de xin bianhua [New changes in Tianjin tones]. In The joy of research: A festschrift in honor of Professor William S.-Y. Wang on his seventieth birthday, ed. Feng Shi, and Zhongwei Shen, 176–188. Tianjin: Nankai University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, Samuel H. 1993. Taiyu biandiao de xinli texing [On the psychological status of Taiwanese tone sandhi]. Tsinghua Xuebao [Tsinghua Journal of Chinese Studies] 23: 175–192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, Jia-Ling. 2002. Youxuanlun he Tianjinhua de liandu biandiao ji qingsheng [Optimality Theory and tone sandhi and neutral tone in Tianjin dialect]. Zhongguo Yuwen [Studies of the Chinese Language] 2002(4): 363–371.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warner, Natasha, Allard Jongman, Joan Sereno, and Rachèl Kemper. 2004. Incomplete neutralization of sub-phonemic durational differences in production and perception of Dutch. Journal of Phonetics 32: 251–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wee, Lian-Hee. 2004. Inter-tier correspondence theory. PhD dissertation, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, NJ.

  • Wilson, Colin. 2006. Learning phonology with substantive bias: An experimental and computational study of velar palatalization. Cognitive Science 30(5): 945–982.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, Yi. 1997. Contextual tonal variations in Mandarin. Journal of Phonetics 25: 61–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, Yi. 2005. TimeNormalizedF0. Praat script. http://www.phon.ucl.ac.uk/home/yi/tools.html. Accessed 1 Dec 2005.

  • Yang, Zi-Xiang, He-Tong Guo, and Xiang-Dong Shi. 1999. Tianjinhua Yindang [The sound system of Tianjin dialect]. Shanghai: Shanghai Education Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yu, Alan C.L. 2007. Understanding near mergers: The case of morphological tone in Cantonese. Phonology 24: 187–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yue-Hashimoto, Anne O. 1987. Tone sandhi across Chinese dialects. In Wang Li memorial volumes, English volume, ed. Chinese Language Society of Hong Kong, 445–474. Hong Kong: Joint Publishing Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, Jie. 2002. The effects of duration and sonority on contour tone distribution: A typological survey and formal analysis. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, Jie. 2007. A directional asymmetry in Chinese tone sandhi systems. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 16: 259–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, Jie. 2010. Issues in the analysis of Chinese tone. Language and Linguistics Compass 4(12): 1137–1153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, Jie. 2014a. Tones, tonal phonology, and tone sandhi. In The handbook of Chinese linguistics, ed. C.-T.James Huang, Y.-H.Audrey Li, and Andrew Simpson, 443–464. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, Jie. 2014b. Tone sandhi. In Oxford bibliographies in linguistics, ed. Mark Aronoff. New York: Oxford University Press. http://www.oxfordbibliographies.com/view/document/obo-9780199772810/obo-9780199772810-0160.xml. Accessed 15 July 2014.

  • Zhang, Jie, and Yuwen Lai. 2008. Phonological knowledge beyond the lexicon in Taiwanese double reduplication. In Interfaces in Chinese phonology: Festschrift in honor of Matthew Y. Chen on his 70th birthday, ed. Yuchau E. Hsiao, Hui-Chuan Hsu, Lian-Hee Wee, and Dah-An Ho, 183–222. Taipei: Academia Sinica.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, Jie, and Yuwen Lai. 2010. Testing the role of phonetic knowledge in Mandarin tone sandhi. Phonology 27(1): 153–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, Jie, and Jiang Liu. 2011. Tone sandhi and tonal coarticulation in Tianjin Chinese. Phonetica 68(3): 161–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, Jie, and Yuanliang Meng. 2012. Structure-dependent tone sandhi in real and nonce words in Shanghai Wu. In Proceedings of the 3rd international symposium on tonal aspects of languages, ed. Gu Wentao. Nanjing: Nanjing Normal University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, Jie, Yuwen Lai, and Craig Sailor. 2009. Opacity, phonetics, and frequency in Taiwanese tone sandhi. In Current issues in unity and diversity of languages: Collection of papers selected from the 18th International Congress of Linguists, ed. Manghyu Pak, 3019–3038. Seoul: Linguistic Society of Korea.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, Jie, Yuwen Lai, and Craig Sailor. 2011. Modeling Taiwanese speakers’ knowledge of tone sandhi in reduplication. Lingua 121(2): 181–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhao, Yuan, and Dan Jurafsky. 2009. The effect of lexical frequency and Lombard reflex on tone hyperarticulation. Journal of Phonetics 37: 231–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhu, Xiaonong. 2004. Jipin guiyihua — ruhe chuli shengdiao de suiji chayi? [F0 normalization — How to deal with between-speaker tonal variations?]. Yuyan Kexue [Linguistic Sciences] 3(2): 3–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zuraw, Kie. 2000. Patterned exceptions in phonology. PhD dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles.

  • Zuraw, Kie. 2007. The role of phonetic knowledge in phonological patterning: Corpus and survey evidence from Tagalog infixation. Language 83: 277–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zuraw, Kie. 2010. A model of lexical variation and the grammar with application to Tagalog nasal substitution. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 28: 417–472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We are indebted to Ping Wang, Xiaoyu Zeng, and Feng Shi at Nankai University for hosting us during data collection and discussing various aspects of this project with us. We also thank Geng Wang for serving as our Tianjin language consultant and the speakers of Tianjin who participated in our experiment. We are grateful to the participants at GLOW-Asia 8 and the second Pan-American/Iberian Meeting on Acoustics, especially James Myers, Doug Whalen, and Charles Yang, for their comments on this research. We, however, remain fully responsible for the opinions expressed here. This research was supported by the National Science Foundation grant BCS-0750773 and the University of Kansas General Research Fund 2301166.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Jie Zhang or Jiang Liu.

Electronic supplementary material

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Zhang, J., Liu, J. The productivity of variable disyllabic tone sandhi in Tianjin Chinese. J East Asian Linguist 25, 1–35 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10831-015-9135-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10831-015-9135-0

Keywords

Navigation