Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

How Are Moral Foundations Associated with Climate-Friendly Consumption?

  • Articles
  • Published:
Journal of Agricultural and Environmental Ethics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We examined whether differences in climate-friendly choices between the supporters of left-wing and right-wing ideologies are based on different moral foundations. Moreover, we compared general and issue-specific endorsement of moral foundations applied to climate change. Study 1 examined the endorsement of general moral foundations of university students living in Finland (N = 272). Individualizing foundations were associated with increased climate-friendly choices and binding foundations were associated with decreased climate-friendly choices; the endorsement of moral foundations made the effect of political orientation disappear. In Study 2 we developed and tested an issue-specific measure of moral foundations (N = 350). The issue-specific endorsement of both types of foundations was directly associated with increased climate-friendly consumption. Binding foundations were associated with the avoidance of climate-friendly choices through right-wing orientation. These findings increase our understanding of differences between general and issue-specific moral concerns and their association to political orientation and pro-environmental behavior.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The current version of the Moral Foundations Questionnaire measures five foundations. http://www.moralfoundations.org/questionnaires.

  2. In Study 1, MFQ was not adjusted to the climate change context. The goal of the translation was to be faithful to the original MFQ and, at the same time, be meaningful in the Finnish context.

References

  • Adger, W. N., Dessai, S., Goulden, M., Hulme, M., Lorenzoni, I., Nelson, D. R., et al. (2009). Are there social limits to adaptation to climate change? Climatic Change, 93, 335–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arbuckle, J. L. (2011). IBM ® SPSS ® Amos™ 21 user’s guide. Spring House, PA: AMOS Development Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arnett, J. J., Ramos, K., & Jensen, L. A. (2001). Ideological views in emerging adulthood: Balancing autonomy and community. Journal of Adult Development, 8, 69–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bamberg, S. (2003). How does environmental concern influence specific environmentally related behaviors? A new answer to an old question. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 23, 21–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bamberg, S., & Möser, G. (2007). Twenty years after Hines, Hungerford, and Tomera: A new meta-analysis of psychosocial determinants of pro-environmental behavior. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 27, 14–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 238–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blok, V., Wesselink, R., Studynka, O., & Kemp, R. (2015). Encouraging sustainability in the workplace: A survey on the pro-environmental behaviour of university employees. Journal of Cleaner Production, 106, 55–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen & S. J. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136–162). California: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carmines, E. G., & McIver, J. P. (1981). Analyzing models with unobserved variables. In G. W. Bohrnstedt & E. Borgatta (Eds.), Social measurement: Current issues (pp. 65–115). Beverly Hills: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chung, J., & Monroe, G. S. (2003). Exploring social desirability bias. Journal of Business Ethics, 44, 291–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1995). Constructing validity: Basic issues in objective scale development. Psychological Assessment, 7, 309–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corraliza, J. A., & Berenguer, J. (2000). Environmental values, beliefs, and actions: A situational approach. Environment and Behavior, 32, 832–848.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emler, N. (1999). Moral character. In V. Derlaga, B. Winstead, & W. Jones (Eds.), Personality: Contemporary theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 376–404). Chicago: Nelson.

    Google Scholar 

  • European Commission (2011). Climate change report. Special eurobarometer, 372. Retrieved from http://ec.europa.eu/public_opinion/archives/ebs/ebs_372_en.pdf.

  • Feinberg, M., & Willer, R. (2013). The moral roots of environmental attitudes. Psychological Science, 24, 56–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feygina, I., Jost, J. T., & Goldsmith, R. E. (2010). System justification, the denial of global warming, and the prospect of “system-sanctioned” change. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 326–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flere, S., & Lavrič, M. (2008). On the validity of cross-cultural social studies using student samples. Field Methods, 20, 399–412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, J., Haidt, J., Koleva, S., Motyl, M., Iyer, R., Wojcik, S., & Ditto, P. H. (2012). Moral foundations theory: The pragmatic validity of moral pluralism. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 55–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, J., Haidt, J., & Nosek, B. A. (2009). Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 1029–1046.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, J., Nosek, B. A., Haidt, J., Iyer, R., Koleva, S., & Ditto, P. H. (2011). Mapping the moral domain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 366–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J. (2010). How to translate the MFQ. Retrieved from http://www.yourmorals.org/blog/2010/01/how-to-translate-the-mfq/.

  • Haidt, J., & Graham, J. (2007). When morality opposes justice: Conservatives have moral intuitions that liberals may not recognize. Social Justice Research, 20, 98–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunter, J. D. (1991). Culture wars. The struggle to define America. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • IPCC (the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change). (2014). Climate change 2014: Synthesis report. Geneva: IPCC.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Iyer, R., Koleva, S., Graham, J., Ditto, P., & Haidt, J. (2012). Understanding libertarian morality: The psychological dispositions of self-identified libertarians. PLoS One, 7, e42366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jamieson, D. (2009). Climate change, responsibility, and justice. Science and Engireening Ethics, 16, 431–445.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, K. M., Iyer, R., Wojcik, S. P., Vaisey, S., Miles, A., Chu, V., & Graham, J. (2014). Ideology-specific patterns of moral indifference predict intentions not to vote. Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy, 14, 61–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, T. M. (1991). Ethical decision making by individuals in organizations: An issue-contingent model. The Academy of Management Review, 16, 366–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, M. D., & Song, G. (2014). Making sense of climate change: How story frames shape cognition. Political Psychology, 35, 447–476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (1984). Lisrel VI. Analysis of linear structural relationships by maximum likelihood, instrumental variables, and least squares methods. Mooresville: Scientific Software.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Exceptions that prove the rule—using a theory of motivated social cognition to account for ideological incongruities and political anomalies: Reply to Greenberg and Jonas (2003). Psychological Bulletin, 129, 383–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Juujärvi, S. (2005). Care and justice in real-life moral reasoning. Journal of Adult Development, 12, 199–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kenny, D. A. (2015). Measuring model fit. Retrieved from http://davidakenny.net/cm/fit.htm.

  • Koleva, S. P., Graham, J., Iyer, R., Ditto, P. H., & Haidt, J. (2012). Tracing the threads: How five moral concerns (especially Purity) help explain culture war attitudes. Journal of Research in Personality, 46, 184–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kugler, M., Jost, J. T., & Noorbaloochi, S. (2014). Another look at moral foundations theory: Do authoritarianism and social dominance orientation explain liberal-conservative differences in “moral” intuitions? Social Justice Research, 27, 413–431.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kvaløy, B., Finseraas, H., & Listhaug, O. (2012). The publics’ concern for global warming: A cross-national study of 47 countries. Journal of Peace Research, 49, 11–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leiserowitz, A. (2006). Climate change risk perception and policy preferences: The role of affect imagery and values. Climatic Change, 77, 45–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., & Williams, J. (2004). Confidence limits for the indirect effect: Distribution of the product and resampling methods. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39, 99–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mäkiniemi, J.-P., Pirttilä-Backman, A.-M., & Pieri, M. (2013). The endorsement of the moral foundations in food-related moral thinking in three European countries. Journal of Agricultural and Environmental Ethics, 26, 771–786.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mäkiniemi, J.-P., & Vainio, A. (2013). Moral intensity and climate-friendly food choices. Appetite, 66, 54–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mäkiniemi, J.-P., & Vainio, A. (2014). Barriers to climate-friendly food choices among young adults in Finland. Appetite, 74, 12–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mandic, S. (2008). Home-leaving and its structural determinants in Western and Eastern Europe: An exploratory study. Housing Studies, 23, 615–637.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markowitz, E. M. (2012). Is climate change an ethical issue? Exploring young adults’ beliefs about climate and morality. Climatic Change, 114, 479–495.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markowitz, E. M., & Shariff, A. F. (2012). Climate change and moral judgment. Nature Climate Change, 2, 243–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McAdams, D. P., Albaugh, M., Farber, E., Daniels, J., Logan, R., & Olson, B. (2008). Family metaphors and moral intuitions: How conservatives and liberals narrate their lives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 978–990.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCright, A. M., & Dunlap, R. E. (2011a). Cool dudes: The denial of climate change among conservative white males in the United States. Global Environmental Change, 21, 1163–1172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCright, A. M., & Dunlap, R. E. (2011b). The politicization of climate change and polarization in the American public’s views of global warming, 2001–2010. The Sociological Quarterly, 52, 155–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MoralFoundations.org (2013). MFQ30 (Moral foundations questionnaire). Retrieved from http://moralfoundations.org/questionnaires.

  • Myyry, L., & Helkama, K. (2007). Socio-cognitive conflict, emotions and complexity of thought in real-life morality. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 48, 247–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nilsson, A., & Erlandsson, A. (2015). The Moral Foundations taxonomy: Structural validity and relation to political ideology in Sweden. Personality and Individual Differences, 76, 28–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nilsson, A., von Borgstede, C., & Biel, A. (2004). Willingness to accept climate change strategies: The effect of values and norms. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24, 267–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oreskes, N. (2014). The scientific consensus on climate change: How do we know we’re not wrong? In J. F. C. DiMento & P. Doughman (Eds.), Climate change: What it means for us, our children, and our grandchildren (2nd ed., pp. 105–148). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poortinga, W., Steg, L., & Vlek, C. (2004). Values, environmental concern, and environmental behavior: A study into household energy use. Environment and Behavior, 36, 70–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seppälä, J., Mäenpää, I., Koskela, S., Mattila, T., Nissinen, A., Katajajuuri, J.-M., et al. (2011). An assessment of greenhouse gas emissions and material flows caused by the Finnish economy using the ENVIMAT model. Journal of Cleaner Production, 19, 1833–1841.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stern, P. C. (2000). New environmental theories: Toward a coherent theory of environmentally significant behavior. Journal of Social Issues, 56, 407–424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Straus, M. A. (2009). The national context effect: An empirical test of the validity of cross-national research using unrepresentative samples. Cross-Cultural Research, 43, 183–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: New procedures and recommendations. Psychological Methods, 7, 422–445.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tikir, A., & Lehmann, B. (2011). Climate change, theory of planned behavior and values: A structural equation model with mediation analysis. Climatic Change, 104, 389–402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vainio, A. (2011). Religious conviction, morality and social convention among Finnish adolescents. Journal of Moral Education, 40, 73–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vainio, A. (2015). Finnish moral landscapes: A comparison of nonreligious, liberal religious and conservative religious adolescents. In L. A. Jensen (Ed.), Moral development in a global world: Research from a cultural-developmental perspective (pp. 46–68). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Vainio, A., Mäkiniemi, J.-P., & Paloniemi, R. (2014). System justification and the perception of food risks. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 17, 510–524.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vainio, A., & Paloniemi, R. (2013). Does belief matter in climate change action? Public Understanding of Science, 22, 382–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vaisey, S. (2014). The “attitudinal fallacy” is a fallacy: Why we need many methods to study culture. Sociological Methods and Research, 43, 227–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Leeuwen, F., & Park, J. H. (2009). Perceptions of social dangers, moral foundations, and political orientation. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 169–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vermeir, I., & Verbeke, W. (2008). Sustainable food consumption among young adults in Belgium: Theory of planned behaviour and the role of confidence and values. Ecological Economics, 64, 542–553.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, C. R., & Federico, C. M. (2013). Moral foundations and heterogeneity in ideological preferences. Political Psychology, 34, 107–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, J. C., & Baril, G. (2011). The role of cognitive resources in determining our moral intuitions: Are we all liberals by heart. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 1007–1012.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Annukka Vainio.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Vainio, A., Mäkiniemi, JP. How Are Moral Foundations Associated with Climate-Friendly Consumption?. J Agric Environ Ethics 29, 265–283 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10806-016-9601-3

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10806-016-9601-3

Keywords

Navigation