Selective Pragmatic Impairment in Autism Spectrum Disorder: Indirect Requests Versus Irony
Abstract
Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is often described as being characterised by a uniform pragmatic impairment. However, recent evidence suggests that some areas of pragmatic functioning are preserved. This study seeks to determine to which extent context-based derivation of non-linguistically encoded meaning is functional in ASD. We compare the performance of 24 adults with ASD, and matched neuro-typical adults in two act-out pragmatic tasks. The first task examines generation of indirect request interpretations, and the second the comprehension of irony. Intact contextual comprehension of indirect requests contrasts with marked difficulties in understanding irony. These results suggest that preserved pragmatics in ASD is limited to egocentric processing of context, which does not rely on assumptions about the speaker’s mental states.
Keywords
Autism Pragmatics Communication Irony Indirect speech acts Request Eye-tracking Executive function Social motivationNotes
Acknowledgments
First and foremost, we warmly thank all our participants, as well as participating institutions. We greatly benefited from comments and suggestions made on previous versions of this paper by the acting editor, Joshua J. Diehl and three anonymous reviewers. We also thank Véronique Ginsburg for her help in data acquisition. Finally, we are very grateful to the Foundation Jean-François Peterbroeck and to the Foundation ULB for their continuous support of the ACTE research group.
Author Contributions
GD designed the experiment, recruited participants, ran testing sessions, took part in the result analyses and in the redaction of the paper. FP recruited participants and ran testing sessions. NR designed the experiment and took part in the result analyses. PG recruited participants and ran testing sessions. MK designed the experiment, analysed the results and wrote the paper.
Compliance with Ethical Standards
Ethical Approval
All procedures in this study were approved by the ethics committee of Erasme Hospital in accordance with the 1964 declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments. All adult participants provided informed consent. Adolescent participants provided informed assent with their parents providing informed consent.
References
- Adams, N. C., & Jarrold, C. (2009). Inhibition and the validity of the Stroop task for children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(8), 1112–1121.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5). Wasington: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
- Andrés-Roqueta, C., & Katsos, N. (2017). The contribution of grammar, vocabulary and theory of mind in pragmatic language competence in children with autistic spectrum disorders. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 996.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Attwood, A. (2015). The complete guide to asperger’s syndrome. London: Jessica Kingsley.Google Scholar
- Baron-Cohen, S. (1992). Out of sight or out of mind? Another look at deception in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33(7), 1141–1155.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Baron-Cohen, S. (2000). Theory of mind and autism: A fifteen year review. Understanding other minds. Perspectives from developmental cognitive science. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
- Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empathy quotient: An investigation of adults with asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(21), 63–75.Google Scholar
- Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 15–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255–278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 11–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Berg, E. A. (1948). A simple objective technique for measuring flexibility in thinking. Journal of General Psychology, 39(1), 15–22.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Bramham, J., Ambery, F., Young, S., Morris, R., Russell, A., Xenitidis, K., et al. (2009). Executive functioning differences between adutls with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autistic spectrum disorder in initiation, planning and strategy formation. Autism, 13, 245–264.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Breheny, R. (2006). Communication and folk psychology. Mind and Language, 21(1), 74–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Brock, J., Norbury, C. F., Einav, S., & Nation, K. (2008). Do individuals with autism process words in context? Evidence from language-mediated eye-mouvements. Cognition, 108, 896–904.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Bryant, G. A. (2012). Is verbal irony special? Language and Linguistics Compass, 6, 673–685.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Chevallier, C., Kohls, G., Troiani, V., Brodkin, E. S., & Schultz, R. T. (2012). The social motivation theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 164, 231–239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Chevallier, C., Noveck, I., Happé, F. G. E., & Wilson, D. (2011). What’s in a voice? Prosody as a test case for the theory of mind account of autism. Neuropsychologia, 49(3), 507–517.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Chevallier, C., Wilson, D., Happé, F. G. E., & Noveck, I. (2010). Scalar inferences in autism spectrum disorders scalar inferences in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 1104–1017.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Clark, H. H., & Lucy, P. (1975). Understanding what is meant from what is said: A study in conversationally conveyed requests. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 56–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Climie, E. A., & Pexman, P. M. (2008). Eye gaze provides a window on children’s understanding of verbal irony. Journal of Cognition and Development, 9(3), 257–285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Colich, N. L., Wang, A.-T., Rudie, J. D., Hernandez, L. M., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Dapretto, M. (2012). Atypical neural processing of ironic and sincere remarks in children and adolescent with autism spectrum disorders. Metaphor and Symbol, 27(1), 70–92.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Currie, C. E., Elton, R. A., Todd, J., & Platt, S. (1997). Indicators of socioeconomic status for adolescents: The WHO Health Behaviour in School-aged Children Survey. Health Education Research, 12(3), 385–397.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Deliens, G., Antoniou, K., Clin, E., Ostashchenko, E., & Kissine, M. (2018). Context, facial expression and prosody in irony processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 99, 35–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- de Villiers, J., Fine, J., Ginsberg, G., Vaccarella, L., & Szatmari, P. (2006). Brief report: A scale for rating conversational impairment in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(7), 1375–1380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Filippova, E., & Astington, J. W. (2008). Further development in social reasoning revealed in discourse irony understanding. Child Development, 79(1), 26–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Gerrig, R. J., & Goldvarg, Y. (2000). Additive effects in the perception of sarcasm: Situational disparity and echoic mention. Metaphor and Symbol, 15(4), 197–208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Geurts, H. M., Corbett, B., & Solomon, M. (2009). The paradox of cognitive flexibility in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(2), 74–82.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Gibbs, R. W. (2002). A new look at literal meaning in understanding what is said and implicated. Journal of Pragmatics, 34(4), 457–486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Glucksberg, S., Gildea, P., & Bookin, H. B. (1982). On understanding nonliteral speech: Can people ignore metaphors? Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21(1), 85–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & J. L. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and semantics: Speech acts (pp. 41–58). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
- Happé, F. G. E. (1993). Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism. A test of relevance theory. Cognition, 48(2), 101–119.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Happé, F. G. E. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Development, 66(3), 843–855.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Heavey, L., Phillips, W., Baron-Cohen, S., & Rutter, M. (2000). The awkward moments test: A naturalistic measure of social understanding in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 395–406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hermann, I., Haser, V., Van Elst, L., Ebert, D., Müller-Feldmeth, D., Riedel, A., et al. (2013). Automatic metaphor processing in adults with Asperger syndrome: A metaphor interference effect task. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 263, S177–S187.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Hill, E. (2004). Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(1), 26–32.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Jary, M., & Kissine, M. (2014). Imperatives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Joliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). The strange story test: A replication with high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(5), 395–406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kaland, N., Moller-Nielsen, A., Callesen, K., Mortensen, E. L., Gottlieb, D., & Smith, L. (2002). A new ’advanced’ test of Theory of Mind: Evidence from children and adolescents with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 43(4), 517–528.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kim, S. H., Paul, R., Tager-Flusberg, H., Lord, C., Volkmar, F. R., Paul, R., et al. (2014). Language and communication in autism language and communication in autism. In F. R. Volkmar, R. Paul, S. J. Rogers, & K. A. Pelphrey (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (4th ed., pp. 230–262). Hoboken: Wiley.Google Scholar
- Kissine, M. (2012). Pragmatics, cognitive flexibility and autism spectrum disorders. Mind and Language, 27, 1–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kissine, M. (2013). From utterances to speech acts. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kissine, M. (2016). Pragmatics as metacognitive control. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 2057.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Kissine, M., Cano-Chervel, J., Carlier, S., De Brabanter, P., Ducenne, L., Pairon, M.-C., et al. (2015). Children with autism understand indirect speech acts: Evidence from a semi-structured act-out task. PLoS ONE, 10, e0142191.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Kissine, M., De Brabanter, P., & Leybaert, J. (2012). The interpretation of requests in children with autism: The effect of the sentence-type. Autism, 16, 523–532.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Kowatch, K., Whalen, J. M., & Pexman, P. M. (2013). Irony comprehension in action: A new test of processing verbal irony. Discourse Processes, 50, 301–315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kreuz, R. J. (2000). The production and processing of verbal irony. Metaphor and Symbol, 15(1–2), 99–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kreuz, R. J., & Link, K. E. (2002). Asymmetries in the use of verbal irony. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 21(2), 127–143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Lai, C. L. E., Lau, Z., Lui, S. S. Y., Lok, E., Tam, V., Chan, Q., et al. (2017). Meta-analysis of neuropsychological measures of executive functioning in children and adolescents with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 10(5), 911–939.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Landry, O., & Al-Taie, S. (2016). A meta-analysis of the wisconsin card sort task in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(4), 1220–1235.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Le Couteur, A., Lord, C., & Rutter, M. (2003). The autism diagnostic interview-revised (ADI-R). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
- Lenth, R. (2016). Least-squares means: The R package lsmeans. Journal of Statistical Software, 69(1), 1–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. (2012). Autism diagnostic observation schedule (ADOS) (2nd ed.). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
- Loukusa, S., Leinonen, E., Kuusikko, S., Jussila, K., Mattila, M.-L., Ryder, N., et al. (2006). Use of context in pragmatic language comprehension by children with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(6), 1049–1059.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- MacKay, G., & Shaw, A. (2005). A comparative study of figurative language in children with autistic spectrum disorders. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 20(1), 13–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Martin, I., & McDonald, S. (2003). Weak coherence, no theory of mind, or executive dysfunction? Solving the puzzle of pragmatic language disorders. Brain and Language, 85, 451–466.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Martin, I., & McDonald, S. (2004). An exploration of causes of non-literal language problems in individuals with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(3), 311–328.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Mary, A., Slama, H., Mousty, P., Massat, I., Capiau, T., Drabs, V., et al. (2016). Executive and attentional contributions to theory of mind deficit in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Child Neuropsychology, 22(3), 345–365.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Monetta, L., & Champagne, M. (2004). Processuscognitifs sous jacents déterminant les troubles de la communication verbale chez les cérébrolésés droits. Réeducation orthophonique, 42(219), 27–41.Google Scholar
- Norbury, C. F. (2005). Barking up the wrong tree? Lexical ambiguity resolution in children with language impairments and autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 90, 141–171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Norbury, C. F. (2005). The relationship between theory of mind and metaphor: Evidence from children with language impairment and autistic spectrum disorder. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23(3), 383–399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Ozonoff, S. (1997). Components of executive function in autism and other disorders. In J. Russell (Ed.), Autism as an executive disorder (pp. 179–211). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
- Ozonoff, S., & Miller, J. N. (1996). An exploration of right-hemisphere contributions to the pragmatic impairments of autism. Brain and Language, 52, 411–434.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1991). Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: Relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 32(7), 1081–1105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Ozonoff, S., South, M., & Provencal, S. (2005). Executive functions. In F. R. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (3rd ed., pp. 606–627). Hoboken: Wiley.Google Scholar
- Paul, R., & Cohen, D. J. (1985). Comprehension of indirect requests in adults with autistic disorders and mental retardation. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 28(4), 475–479.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Paul, R., Orlovski, S. M., Marcinko, H. C., & Volkmar, F. (2008). Conversational behaviors in youth with high-functioning ASD and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(1), 115–125.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Perkins, M. R. (2007). Pragmatic impairment. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Perner, J., & Winner, E. (1985). “John thinks, that Mary thinks that…” attribution of second-order belief by 5- to 10-year-old children. Journal of Child Experimental Psychology, 39, 437–471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Persicke, A., Tarbox, J., Ranick, J., & St. Clair, M. (2013). Teaching children with autism to detect and respond to sarcasm. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 71, 193–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Pexman, P. M., Rostad, K. R., McMorris, C. A., Climie, E. A., Stowkowy, J., & Glenwright, M. R. (2011). Processing of ironic language in children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(8), 1097–1112.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Pijnacker, J., Hagoort, P., Buitelaar, J., Teunisse, J. P, & Geurts, B. (2009). Pragmatic inferences in high-functioning adults with autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 607–618.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- R Core Team. (2016). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
- Recanati, F. (2004). Literal meaning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
- Russell, J., & Hughes, C. (1994). Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism. Neuropsychologia, 32(4), 477–492.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Ruytenbeek, N., Ostashchenko, E., & Kissine, M. (2017). Indirect request processing, sentence-types and illocutionary forces. Journal of Pragmatics, 119, 46–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Searle, J. R. (1975). Indirect speech acts. In P. Cole & J. L. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and semantics: Speech acts (Vol. 3, pp. 59–82). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
- Senju, A., Southgate, V., Miura, Y., Matsui, T., Hasegawa, T., Tojo, Y., et al. (2010). Absence of spontaneous action anticipation by false belief attribution in children with autism spectrum disorders. Development and Psychopathology, 22, 353–360.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 298(1089), 199–209.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Shintel, H., & Keysar, B. (2009). Less is more: A minimalist account of joint action in communication. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1, 260–273.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (2002). Pragmatics, modularity and mind-reading. Mind and Language, 17, 3–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Surian, L., Baron-Cohen, S., & Van der Lely, H. (1996). Are children with autism deaf to Gricean maxims? Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 1(1), 55–71.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Wang, A. T., Lee, S. S., Sigman, M., & Dapretto, M. (2006). Neural basis of irony comprehension in children with autism: The role of prosody and context. Brain, 129(4), 932–943.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Wearing, C. (2010). Autism, metaphor and Revelance theory. Mind and Language, 25(2), 196–216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-IV) (4th ed., Vol. 22). San Antonio: NCS Pearson.Google Scholar
- Yirmiya, N., Erel, O., Shaked, M., & Solomonica-Levi, D. (1998). Meta-analyses comparing theory of mind abilities of individuals with autism, individuals with mental retardation, and normally developing individuals. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 283–307.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Zelazo, P. D., Jacques, S., Burack, J. A., & Frye, D. (2002). The relation between theory of mind and rule use: Evidence from persons with autism-spectrum disorders. Infant and Child Development, 11, 171–195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar