Skip to main content
Log in

Right Temporoparietal Gray Matter Predicts Accuracy of Social Perception in the Autism Spectrum

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Individuals with an autism spectrum disorder (ASD) show hallmark deficits in social perception. These difficulties might also reflect fundamental deficits in integrating visual signals. We contrasted predictions of a social perception and a spatial–temporal integration deficit account. Participants with ASD and matched controls performed two tasks: the first required spatiotemporal integration of global motion signals without social meaning, the second required processing of socially relevant local motion. The ASD group only showed differences to controls in social motion evaluation. In addition, gray matter volume in the temporal–parietal junction correlated positively with accuracy in social motion perception in the ASD group. Our findings suggest that social–perceptual difficulties in ASD cannot be reduced to deficits in spatial–temporal integration.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abell, F., Happe, F., & Frith, U. (2000). Do triangles play tricks? Attribution of mental states to animated shapes in normal and abnormal development. Journal of Cognitive Development, 15, 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abell, F., Krams, M., Ashburner, J., Passingham, R., Friston, K., Frackowiak, R., et al. (1999). The neuroanatomy of autism: A voxel-based whole brain analysis of structural scans. NeuroReport, 10(8), 1647–1651.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adolphs, R. (2003). Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(3), 165–178.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Amaral, D. G., Schumann, C. M., & Nordahl, C. W. (2008). Neuroanatomy of autism. Trends in Neurosciences, 31(3), 137–145.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Annaz, D., Remington, A., Milne, E., Coleman, M., Campbell, R., Thomas, M. S. C., et al. (2010). Development of motion processing in children with autism. Developmental Science, 13(6), 826–838.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ashburner, J., & Friston, K. J. (2000). Voxel-based morphometry—The methods. NeuroImage, 11(6), 805–821.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, A. P. (2009). Impaired recognition of emotions from body movements is associated with elevated motion coherence thresholds in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia, 47(13), 3023–3029.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a theory of mind? Cognition, 21, 37–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(1), 5–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bastiaansen, J. A., Meffert, H., Hein, S., Huizinga, P., Ketelaars, C., Pijnenborg, M., et al. (2011). Diagnosing autism spectrum disorders in adults: The use of Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) module 4. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41, 1256–1266.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Behrmann, M., Thomas, C., & Humphreys, K. (2006). Seeing it differently: Visual processing in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(6), 258–264.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bertone, A., Mottron, L., Jelenic, P., & Faubert, J. (2005). Enhanced and diminished visuo-spatial information processing in autism depends on stimulus complexity. Brain, 128(10), 2430–2441.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bickart, K. C., Wright, C. I., Dautoff, R. J., Dickerson, B. C., & Barrett, L. F. (2011). Amygdala volume and social network size in humans. Nature Neuroscience, 14, 163–164.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blake, R., Turner, L., Smoski, M., Pozdol, S., & Stone, W. (2003). Visual recognition of biological motion is impaired in children with autism. Psychological Science, 14(2), 151–157.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blanke, O. (2005). The out-of-body experience: Disturbed self-processing at the temporo-parietal junction. The Neuroscientist, 11(1), 16–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boddaert, N., Chabane, N., Gervais, H., Good, C., Bourgeois, M., Plumet, M.-H., et al. (2004). Superior temporal sulcus anatomical abnormalities in childhood autism: A voxel-based morphometry MRI study. NeuroImage, 23(1), 364–369.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brambilla, P., Nicoletti, M. A., Sassi, R. B., Mallinger, A. G., Frank, E., Kupfer, D. J., et al. (2003). Magnetic resonance imaging study of corpus callosum abnormalities in patients with bipolar disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 54(11), 1294–1297.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brieber, S., Neufang, S., Bruning, N., Kamp-Becker, I., Remschmidt, H., Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., et al. (2007). Structural brain abnormalities in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder and patients with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(12), 1251–1258.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, M. J., Scott, F. J., Fox, S., & Pye, J. (2004). Gestalt processing in autism: Failure to process perceptual relationships and the implications for contextual understanding. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(3), 459–469.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Button, K. S., Ioannidis, J. P. A., Mokrysz, C., Nosek, B. A., Flint, J., Robinson, E. S. J., et al. (2013). Power failure: Why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14, 365–376.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Castelli, F., Frith, C., Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2002). Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain, 125, 1839–1849.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Castelli, F., Happé, F., Frith, U., Frith, C. (2000). Movement and mind: A functional imaging study of perception and interpretation of complex intentional movement patterns. NeuroImage, 12, 314–325.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cheverud, J. M. (2001). A simple correction for multiple comparisons in interval mapping genome scans. Heredity, 87, 52–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Congiu, S., Schlottmann, A., & Ray, E. (2010). Unimpaired perception of social and physical causality, but impaired perception of animacy in high functioning children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 39–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corbetta, M., Kincade, J., Ollinger, J., McAvoy, M., & Shulman, G. (2000). Voluntary orienting is dissociated from target detection in human posterior parietal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 3(3), 292–297.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corbetta, M., Miezin, F., Dobmeyer, S., Shulman, G., & Petersen, S. (1991). Selective and divided attention during visual discriminations of shape, color, and speed: Functional anatomy by positron emission tomography. The Journal of Neuroscience, 11(8), 2383–2402.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Courchesne, E., Townsend, J., Akshoomoff, N. A., Saitoh, O., Yeung-Courchesne, R., Lincoln, A. J., et al. (1994). Impairment in shifting attention in autistic and cerebellar patients. Behavioral Neuroscience, 108(5), 848–865.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dakin, S., & Frith, U. (2005). Vagaries of visual perception in autism. Neuron, 48(3), 497–507.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Jonge, M. V., Kemner, C., de Haan, E. H., Coppens, J. E., van den Berg, T. J. T. P., & van Engeland, H. (2007). Visual information processing in high-functioning individuals with autism spectrum disorders and their parents. Neuropsychology, 21(1), 65–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Decety, J., & Lamm, C. (2007). The role of the right temporoparietal junction in social interaction: How low-level computational processes contribute to meta-cognition. The Neuroscientist, 13, 580–593.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Del Viva, M. M., Igliozzi, R., Tancredi, R., & Brizzolara, D. (2006). Spatial and motion integration in children with autism. Vision Research, 46(8–9), 1242–1252.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • den Ouden, H. E. M., Frith, U., Frith, C., & Blakemore, S. J. (2005). Thinking about intentions. NeuroImage, 28, 787–796.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Di Martino, A., Ross, K., Uddin, L. Q., Sklar, A. B., Castellanos, F. X., & Milham, M. P. (2009). Functional brain correlates of social and nonsocial processes in autism spectrum disorders: An activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Biological Psychiatry, 65(1), 63–74.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duvernoy, H. (1999). The human brain. New York: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Dziobek, I., Bahnemann, M., Convit, A., & Heekeren, H. R. (2010). The role of the fusiform–amygdala system in the pathophysiology of autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67(4), 397–405.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eickhoff, S. B., Stephan, K. E., Mohlberg, H., Grefkes, C., Fink, G. R., Amunts, K., et al. (2005). A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. NeuroImage, 25(4), 1325–1335.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (1999). Interacting minds—A biological basis. Science, 286(5445), 1692–1695.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, G., Johnson, C. R., & Minshew, N. J. (2001). Attentional processes in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(4), 433–440.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 5–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heider, F., & Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent behaviour. American Journal of Psychology, 57, 243–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, R. P., Ouston, J., & Lee, A. (1988). What’s in a face? The case of autism. British Journal of Psychology, 79(4), 441–453.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hubert, B., Wicker, B., Moore, D. G., Monfardini, E., Duverger, H., Da Fonseca, D., et al. (2007). Brief report: Recognition of emotional and non-emotional biological motion in individuals with autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(7), 1386–1392.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Perception and Psychophysics, 14, 201–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, C. R. G., Swettenham, J., Charman, T., Marsden, A. J. S., Tregay, J., Baird, G., et al. (2011). No evidence for a fundamental visual motion processing deficit in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research, 4(5), 347–357.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kanai, R., Bahrami, B., Duchaine, B., Janik, A., Banissy, M. J., & Rees, G. (2012a). Brain structure links loneliness to social perception. Current Biology, 22, 1975–1979.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kanai, R., Bahrami, B., Roylance, R., & Rees, G. (2012b). Online social network size is reflected in human brain structure. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences, 279(1732), 1327–1334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kanai, R., & Rees, G. (2011). The structural basis of inter-individual differences in human behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12(4), 231–242.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ke, X., Tang, T., Hong, S., Hang, Y., Zou, B., Li, H., et al. (2009). White matter impairments in autism, evidence from voxel-based morphometry and diffusion tensor imaging. Brain Research, 1265, 171–177.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A., & Jones, W. (2006). Attributing social and physical meaning to ambiguous visual displays in individuals with higher-functioning autism spectrum disorders. Brain and Cognition, 61(1), 40–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A., Lin, D. J., Gorrindo, P., Ramsay, G., & Jones, W. (2009). Two-year-olds with autism orient to non-social contingencies rather than biological motion. Nature, 459, 257–261.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koldewyn, K., Whitney, D., & Rivera, S. (2010). The psychophysics of visual motion and global form processing in autism. Brain, 133(2), 599–610.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koldewyn, K., Whitney, D., & Rivera, S. M. (2011). Neural correlates of coherent and biological motion perception in autism. Developmental Science, 14(5), 1075–1088.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kosaka, H., Omori, M., Munesue, T., Ishitobi, M., Matsumura, Y., Takahashi, T., et al. (2010). Smaller insula and inferior frontal volumes in young adults with pervasive developmental disorders. NeuroImage, 50(4), 1357–1363.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lai, G., Pantazatos, S. P., Schneider, H., & Hirsch, J. (2012). Neural systems for speech and song in autism. Brain, 135(3), 961–975.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lai, M.-C., Lombardo, M. V., Pasco, G., Ruigrok, A. N. V., Wheelwright, S. J., Sadek, S. A., et al. (2011). A behavioral comparison of male and female adults with high functioning autism spectrum conditions. PLoS ONE, 6, e20835.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Langdell, T. (1978). Recognition of faces: An approach to the study of autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 19(3), 255–268.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lehrl, S. (1995). Mehrfachwahl-Wortschatz-Intelligenztest MWT-B. Balingen: Spitta Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Jr, Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule—Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–223.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luks, T. L., & Simpson, G. V. (2004). Preparatory deployment of attention to motion activates higher-order motion-processing brain regions. NeuroImage, 22(4), 1515–1522.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mars, R. B., Sallet, J., Schüffelgen, U., Jbabdi, S., Toni, I., & Rushworth, M. F. S. (2011). Connectivity-based subdivisions of the human right “temporoparietal junction area”: Evidence for different areas participating in different cortical networks. Cerebral Cortex, 22(8), 1894–1903.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Milne, E., & Griffiths, H. J. (2007). Visual perception and visual dysfunction in ASD. British and Irish Orthoptic Journal, 4, 15–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milne, E., Swettenham, J., Hansen, P., Campbell, R., Jeffries, H., & Plaisted, K. (2002). High motion coherence thresholds in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(2), 255–263.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Milne, E., White, S., Campbell, R., Swettenham, J., Hansen, P., & Ramus, F. (2006). Motion and form coherence detection in autistic spectrum disorder: Relationship to motor control and 2:4 digit ratio. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(2), 225–237.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Molenberghs, P., Cunnington, R., & Mattingley, J. B. (2012). Brain regions with mirror properties: A meta-analysis of 125 human fMRI studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(1), 341–349.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulières, I., Hubert, B., & Burack, J. (2006). Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: An update, and eight principles of autistic perception. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 27–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nickl-Jockschat, T., Habel, U., Maria Michel, T., Manning, J., Laird, A. R., Fox, P. T., et al. (2011). Brain structure anomalies in autism spectrum disorder—A meta-analysis of VBM studies using anatomic likelihood estimation. Human Brain Mapping, 33, 1470–1489.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nordahl, C. W., Dierker, D., Mostafavi, I., Schumann, C. M., Rivera, S. M., Amaral, D. G., et al. (2007). Cortical folding abnormalities in autism revealed by surface-based morphometry. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27(43), 11725–11735.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oostenveld, R., Fries, P., Maris, E., & Schoffelen, J.-M. (2011). FieldTrip: Open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience, 2011, 1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parron, C., Da Fonseca, D., Santos, A., Moore, D., Monfardini, E., & Deruelle, C. (2008). Recognition of biological motion in children with autistic spectrum disorders. Autism, 12(3), 261–274.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pellicano, E., Gibson, L., Maybery, M., Durkin, K., & Badcock, D. R. (2005). Abnormal global processing along the dorsal visual pathway in autism: A possible mechanism for weak visuospatial coherence? Neuropsychologia, 43(7), 1044–1053.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reitan, R. M. (1958). Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 8(3), 271–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riva, D., Bulgheroni, S., Aquino, D., Di Salle, F., Savoiardo, M., & Erbetta, A. (2011). Basal forebrain involvement in low-functioning autistic children: A voxel-based morphometry study. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 32(8), 1430–1435.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rojas, D. C., Peterson, E., Winterrowd, E., Reite, M. L., Rogers, S. J., & Tregellas, J. R. (2006). Regional gray matter volumetric changes in autism associated with social and repetitive behavior symptoms. BMC Psychiatry, 6(1), 56.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sallet, J., Mars, R. B., Noonan, M. P., Andersson, J. L., O’Reilly, J. X., Jbabdi, S., et al. (2011). Social network size affects neural circuits in macaques. Science, 334(6056), 697–700.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salmond, C. H., Ashburner, J., Connelly, A., Friston, K. J., Gadian, D. G., & Vargha-Khadem, F. (2005). The role of the medial temporal lobe in autistic spectrum disorders. European Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 764–772.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salmond, C. H., Vargha-Khadem, F., Gadian, D. G., de Haan, M., & Baldeweg, T. (2007). Heterogeneity in the patterns of neural abnormality in autistic spectrum disorders: Evidence from ERP and MRI. Cortex, 43(6), 686–699.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sánchez-Cubillo, I., Periáñez, J. A., Acrover-Roig, D., Rodríguez-Sánchez, J. M., Ríos-Lago, M., Tirapu, J., et al. (2009). Construct validity of the Trail Making Test: Role of task-switching, working memory, inhibition/interference control, and visuomotor abilities. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15(03), 438–450.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sanchez-Marin, F. J., & Padilla-Medina, J. A. (2008). A psychophysical test of the visual pathway of children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(7), 1270–1277.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Santiesteban, I., Banissy, M. J., Catmur, C., Bird, G. (2012). Enhancing social ability by stimulating right temporoparietal junction. Current Biology, 22, 2274–2277.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saxe, R. (2006). Uniquely human social cognition. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16(2), 235–239.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saxe, R., & Kanwisher, N. (2003). People thinking about thinking people. The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind”. NeuroImage, 19(4), 1835–1842.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schlögl, A., Keinrath, C., Zimmermann, D., Scherer, R., Leeb, R., & Pfurtscheller, G. (2007). A fully automated correction method of EOG artifacts in EEG recordings. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118(1), 98–104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schultz, J., Friston, K. J., O’Doherty, J., Wolpert, D. M., & Frith, C. D. (2005). Activation in posterior superior temporal sulcus parallels parameter inducing the percept of animacy. Neuron, 45(4), 625–635.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shulman, G. L., Astafiev, S. V., Franke, D., Pope, D. L. W., Snyder, A. Z., McAvoy, M. P., et al. (2009). Interaction of stimulus-driven reorienting and expectation in ventral and dorsal frontoparietal and basal ganglia-cortical networks. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29(14), 4392–4407.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, J., O’Brien, J., Riggs, K., Braddick, O., Atkinson, J., & Wattam-Bell, J. (2000). Motion processing in autism: Evidence for a dorsal stream deficiency. NeuroReport, 11(12), 2765–2767.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sugranyes, G., Kyriakopoulos, M., Corrigall, R., Taylor, E., & Frangou, S. (2011). Autism spectrum disorders and schizophrenia: Meta-analysis of the neural correlates of social cognition. PLoS ONE, 6(10), e25322.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Takarae, Y., Luna, B., Minshew, N. J., & Sweeney, J. A. (2008). Patterns of visual sensory and sensorimotor abnormalities in autism vary in relation to history of early language delay. Journal of the International Neuropsychology Society, 14(6), 980–989.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toal, F., Daly, E. M., Page, L., Deeley, Q., Hallahan, B., Bloemen, O., et al. (2010). Clinical and anatomical heterogeneity in autistic spectrum disorder: A structural MRI study. Psychological Medicine, 40(7), 1171–1181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tremoulet, P. D., & Feldman, J. (2000). Perception of animacy from the motion of a single object. Perception, 29(8), 943–951.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tsermentseli, S., O’Brien, J. M., & Spencer, J. V. (2008). Comparison of form and motion coherence processing in autistic spectrum disorders and dyslexia. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(7), 1201–1210.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Overwalle, F. (2009). Social cognition and the brain: A meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 30(3), 829–858.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vandenbroucke, M. W. G., Steven Scholte, H., Engeland, H., Lamme, V. A. F., & Kemner, C. (2008). Coherent versus component motion perception in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(5), 941–949.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Via, E., Radua, J., Cardoner, N., Happe, F., & Mataix-Cols, D. (2011). Meta-analysis of gray matter abnormalities in autism spectrum disorder: Should Asperger disorder be subsumed under a broader umbrella of autistic spectrum disorder? Archives of General Psychiatry, 68(4), 409–418.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wainwright, J. A., & Bryson, S. E. (1996). Visual-spatial orienting in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26(4), 423–438.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Worsley, K. J., Liao, C. H., Aston, J., Petre, V., Duncan, G. H., Morales, F., et al. (2002). A general statistical analysis for fMRI data. NeuroImage, 15(1), 1–15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yu, K. K., Cheung, C., Chua, S. E., & McAlonan, G. M. (2011). Can Asperger syndrome be distinguished from autism? An anatomic likelihood meta-analysis of MRI studies. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 36(6), 412–421.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zwickel, J., White, S. J., Coniston, D., Senju, A., & Frith, U. (2011). Exploring the building blocks of social cognition: Spontaneous agency perception and visual perspective taking in autism. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6(5), 564–571.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We especially would like to thank all volunteers who participated in this study. Furthermore, we thank C. Reissmann, K. Deazle and A. Daniel for help with recruitment and data assessment, J. Hipp and T.R. Schneider for help with motion coherence task, and I. Peiker for statistical advice. This work was supported by the European Union (HEALTH-F2-2008-200728, ERC-2010-AdG-269716, FP7-ICT-270212). N.D. was supported by a fellowship of the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DA 1358/1-1). J.S. was supported by the Max Planck Society. K.V. was supported by the German Ministry of Research and Education (01 GW 0611). D.S. was supported in part by Bundesministerium für Bildung und Forschung (BMBF 0737/114). O.S. was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG Grant MU1692/2-1).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Nicole David or Johannes Schultz.

Additional information

Nicole David and Johannes Schultz have contributed equally to the paper.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

David, N., Schultz, J., Milne, E. et al. Right Temporoparietal Gray Matter Predicts Accuracy of Social Perception in the Autism Spectrum. J Autism Dev Disord 44, 1433–1446 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-2008-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-2008-3

Keywords

Navigation