Advertisement

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 44, Issue 5, pp 1039–1054 | Cite as

Face and Object Discrimination in Autism, and Relationship to IQ and Age

  • Pamela M. Pallett
  • Shereen J. Cohen
  • Karen R. Dobkins
Original Paper

Abstract

The current study tested fine discrimination of upright and inverted faces and objects in adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) as compared to age- and IQ-matched controls. Discrimination sensitivity was tested using morphed faces and morphed objects, and all stimuli were equated in low-level visual characteristics (luminance, contrast, spatial frequency make-up). Participants with ASD exhibited slight, non-significant impairments in discrimination sensitivity for faces, yet significantly enhanced discrimination sensitivity for objects. The ASD group also showed a protracted development of face and object inversion effects. Finally, for ASD participants, face sensitivity improved with increasing IQ while object sensitivity improved with age. By contrast, for controls, face sensitivity improved with age, but neither face nor object sensitivity was influenced by IQ. These findings suggest that individuals with ASD follow a qualitatively different path in the development of face and object processing abilities.

Keywords

Autism Spectrum Disorders Face processing Object processing Inversion effects Adolescents Development 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This work was funded by NIH/NICHD R01 HD052804-01A2 awarded to Karen Dobkins.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. (2004). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th-TR ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
  2. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
  3. Baranek, G. T. (1999). Autism during infancy: A retrospective video analysis of sensory-motor and social behaviors at 9–12 months of age. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(3), 213–224.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bar-Haim, Y., Shulman, C., Lamy, D., & Reuveni, A. (2006). Attention to eyes and mouth in high-functioning children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 131–137. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0046-1.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Behrmann, M., Thomas, C., & Humphreys, K. (2006). Seeing it differently: Visual processing in autism. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences, 10(6), 258–264. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.05.001.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bookheimer, S. Y., Wang, A. T., Scott, A., Sigman, M., & Dapretto, M. (2008). Frontal contributions to face processing differences in autism: Evidence from fMRI of inverted face processing. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14(6), 922–932. doi: 10.1017/S135561770808140X.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bosworth, R. G., Bartlett, M. S., & Dobkins, K. R. (2006). Image statistics of American Sign Language: Comparison with faces and natural scenes. Journal of the Optical Society of America A: Optics, Image Science, and Vision, 23(9), 2085–2096.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Boucher, J., & Lewis, V. (1992). Unfamiliar face recognition in relatively able autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 33(5), 843–859.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 433–436.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Constantino, J. N., Przybeck, T., Friesen, D., & Todd, R. D. (2000). Reciprocal social behavior in children with and without pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 21(1), 2–11.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Dalton, K. M., Nacewicz, B. M., Johnstone, T., Schaefer, H. S., Gernsbacher, M. A., Goldsmith, H. H., et al. (2005). Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism. Nature Neuroscience, 8(4), 519–526. doi: 10.1038/nn1421.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Damarla, S. R., Keller, T. A., Kana, R. K., Cherkassky, V. L., Williams, D. L., Minshew, N. J., et al. (2010). Cortical underconnectivity coupled with preserved visuospatial cognition in autism: Evidence from an fMRI study of an embedded figures task. Autism Research: Official Journal of the International Society for Autism Research, 3(5), 273–279. doi: 10.1002/aur.153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Deruelle, C., Rondan, C., Gepner, B., & Fagot, J. (2006). Processing of compound visual stimuli by children with autism and Asperger syndrome. International Journal of Psychology, 41(2), 97–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Deruelle, C., Rondan, C., Gepner, B., & Tardif, C. (2004). Spatial frequency and face processing in children with autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(2), 199–210.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Deruelle, C., Rondan, C., Salle-Collemiche, X., Bastard-Rosset, D., & Da Fonseca, D. (2008). Attention to low- and high-spatial frequencies in categorizing facial identities, emotions and gender in children with autism. Brain and Cognition, 66(2), 115–123. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2007.06.001.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Falck-Ytter, T. (2008). Face inversion effects in autism: A combined looking time and pupillometric study. Autism Research: Official Journal of the International Society for Autism Research, 1(5), 297–306. doi: 10.1002/aur.45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Farah, M. J., Wilson, K. D., Drain, M., & Tanaka, J. N. (1998). What is “special” about face perception? Psychological Review, 105(3), 482–498.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Freire, A., Lee, K., & Symons, L. A. (2000). The face-inversion effect as a deficit in the encoding of configural information: Direct evidence. Perception, 29(2), 159–170.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Gauthier, I., Klaiman, C., & Schultz, R. T. (2009). Face composite effects reveal abnormal face processing in Autism spectrum disorders. Vision Research, 49(4), 470–478. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.12.007.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Gepner, B., De Gelder, B., & De Schonen, S. (1996). Face processing in autistics: Evidence for a generalised deficit? Child Neuropsychology: A Journal on Normal and Abnormal Development in Childhood and Adolescence, 2(2), 123–139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Germine, L. T., Duchaine, B., & Nakayama, K. (2011). Where cognitive development and aging meet: Face learning ability peaks after age 30. Cognition, 118(2), 201–210. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.11.002.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Goffaux, V., Peters, J., Haubrechts, J., Schiltz, C., Jansma, B., & Goebel, R. (2011a). From coarse to fine? Spatial and temporal dynamics of cortical face processing. Cerebral Cortex, 21(2), 467–476. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq112.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Goffaux, V., & Rossion, B. (2007). Face inversion disproportionately impairs the perception of vertical but not horizontal relations between features. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(4), 995–1001.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Goffaux, V., van Zon, J., & Schiltz, C. (2011b). The horizontal tuning of face perception relies on the processing of intermediate and high spatial frequencies. Journal of Vision, 11(10). doi:  10.1167/11.10.1.
  25. Golarai, G., Grill-Spector, K., & Reiss, A. L. (2006). Autism and the development of face processing. Clinical Neuroscience Research, 6(3), 145–160. doi: 10.1016/j.cnr.2006.08.001.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Grill-Spector, K., Kushnir, T., Edelman, S., Avidan, G., Itzchak, Y., & Malach, R. (1999). Differential processing of objects under various viewing conditions in the human lateral occipital complex. Neuron, 24, 187–203.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Grossman, R. B., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2008). Reading faces for information about words and emotions in adolescents with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2(4), 681–695. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2008.02.004.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 5–25. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0039-0.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Hauck, M., Fein, D., Maltby, N., Waterhouse, L., & Feinstein, C. (1998). Memory for faces in children with autism. Child Neuropsychology, 4(3), 187–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A., & Gobbini, M. I. (2000). The distributed human neural system for face perception. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences, 4(6), 223–233.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Hole, G. J., George, P. A., & Dunsmore, V. (1999). Evidence for holistic processing for faces viewed as photographic negatives. Perception, 28, 341–359.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Hubl, D., Bolte, S., Feineis-Matthews, S., Lanfermann, H., Federspiel, A., Strik, W., et al. (2003). Functional imbalance of visual pathways indicates alternative face processing strategies in autism. Neurology, 61(9), 1232–1237.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Jarrold, C., & Brock, J. (2004). To match or not to match? Methodological issues in autism-related research. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(1), 81–86.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). Are people with autism and Asperger syndrome faster than normal on the embedded figures test? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 38(5), 527–534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Joseph, R. M., Ehrman, K., McNally, R., & Keehn, B. (2008). Affective response to eye contact and face recognition ability in children with ASD. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14(6), 947–955. doi: 10.1017/S1355617708081344.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Joseph, R. M., & Tanaka, J. (2003). Holistic and part-based face recognition in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 44(4), 529–542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., & Chun, M. M. (1997). The fusiform face area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 17(11), 4302–4311.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Kanwisher, N., Tong, F., & Nakayama, K. (1998). The effect of inversion on the human fusiform face area. Cognition, 68, B1–B11.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Lahaie, A., Mottron, L., Arguin, M., Berthiaume, C., Jemel, B., & Saumier, D. (2006). Face perception in high-functioning autistic adults: Evidence for superior processing of face parts, not for a configural face-processing deficit. Neuropsychology, 20(1), 30–41. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.20.1.30.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Langner, O., Dotsch, R., Bijlstra, G., Wigboldus, D. H. J., Hawk, S. T., & van Knippenberg, A. (2010). Presentation and validation of the Radboud Faces Database. Cognition and Emotion, 24(8), 1377–1388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Leonard, H. C., Annaz, D., Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Johnson, M. H. (2011). Developing spatial frequency biases for face recognition in autism and Williams syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(7), 968–973. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1115-7.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. López, B., Donnelly, N., Hadwin, J. A., & Leekam, S. R. (2004). Face processing in high-functioning adolescents with autism: Evidence for weak central coherence. Visual Cognition, 11(6), 673–688.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H, Jr, Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Macchi Cassia, V., Picozzi, M., Kuefner, D., Bricolo, E., & Turati, C. (2009). Holistic processing for faces and cars in preschool-aged children and adults: Evidence from the composite effect. Developmental science, 12(2), 236–248. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00765.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Macchi Cassia, V., Turati, C., & Schwarzer, G. (2011). Sensitivity to spacing changes in faces and nonface objects in preschool-aged children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109(4), 454–467. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.03.003.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Maestro, S., Muratori, F., Cavallaro, M. C., Pei, F., Stern, D., Golse, B., et al. (2002). Attentional skills during the first 6 months of age in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41(10), 1239–1245. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200210000-00014.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Maurer, D., Le Grand, R., & Mondloch, C. J. (2002). The many faces of configural processing. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences, 6(6), 255–260.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. McCleery, J. P., Akshoomoff, N., Dobkins, K. R., & Carver, L. J. (2009). Atypical faces vs. object processing and hemispheric asymmetries in 10-month-old infants at risk for autism. Biological Psychiatry, 66(10), 950–957.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. McKone, E., & Boyer, B. L. (2006). Sensitivity of 4-year-olds to featural and second-order relational changes in face distinctiveness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 94(2), 134–162.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. McKone, E., Crookes, K., & Kanwisher, N. (2009). The cognitive and neural development of face recognition in humans. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (4th ed., pp. 467–482). USA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  51. McKone, E., Kanwisher, N., & Duchaine, B. C. (2007). Can generic expertise explain special processing for faces? TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences, 11(1), 8–15. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.002.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. McKone, E., & Yovel, G. (2009). Why does picture-plane inversion sometimes dissociate perception of features and spacing in faces, and sometimes not? Toward a new theory of holistic processing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(5), 778–797. doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.5.778.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Morita, T., Kosaka, H., Saito, D. N., Ishitobi, M., Munesue, T., Itakura, S., et al. (2011). Emotional responses associated with self-face processing in individuals with autism spectrum disorders: An fMRI study. Social Neuroscience,. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2011.598945.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. O’Hearn, K., Schroer, E., Minshew, N., & Luna, B. (2010). Lack of developmental improvement on face memory task during adolescence in autism. Neurophsychologia, 48, 3955–3960.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97–113.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Ozonoff, S., Macari, S., Young, G. S., Goldring, S., Thompson, M., & Rogers, S. J. (2008). Atypical object exploration at 12 months of age is associated with autism in a prospective sample. Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice, 12(5), 457–472. doi: 10.1177/1362361308096402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Pallett, P. M., & Dobkins, K. R. (2013). Development of face discrimination abilities, and relationship to magnocellular pathway development, between childhood and adulthood. Visual Neuroscience, First View Articles, 1–12. doi: 10.1017/S0952523813000217.
  58. Pallett, P. M., & MacLeod, D. I. (2011). Seeing faces as objects: No face inversion effect with geometrical discrimination. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 73(2), 504–520. doi: 10.3758/s13414-010-0033-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Pascalis, O., de Vivies, X. D., Anzures, G., Quinn, P. C., Slater, A. M., Tanaka, J. W., et al. (2011). Development of face processing. Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive Science, 2(6), 666–675. doi: 10.1002/wcs.146.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10, 437–442.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Pellicano, E., Rhodes, G., & Peters, M. (2006). Are preschoolers sensitive to configural information in faces? Developmental Science, 9(3), 270–277.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Picozzi, M., Cassia, V. M., Turati, C., & Vescovo, E. (2009). The effect of inversion on 3- to 5-year-olds’ recognition of face and nonface visual objects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102(4), 487–502. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.11.001.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Pierce, K., Müller, R. A., Ambrose, J., Allen, G., & Courchesne, E. (2001). Face processing occurs outside the fusiform ‘face area’ in autism: Evidence from functional MRI. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 124(Pt 10), 2059–2073.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Pitcher, D., Charles, L., Devlin, J. T., Walsh, V., & Duchaine, B. (2009). Triple dissociation of faces, bodies, and objects in extrastriate cortex. Current Biology, 19(4), 319–324. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.01.007.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Riby, D. M., Riby, L. M., & Reay, J. L. (2009). Differential sensitivity to rotations of facial features in the Thatcher illusion. Psychological Reports, 105(3 Pt 1), 721–726.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Richler, J. J., Mack, M. L., Palmeri, T. J., & Gauthier, I. (2011). Inverted faces are (eventually) processed holistically. Vision Research, 51(3), 333–342. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.11.014.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Rinehart, N. J., Bradshaw, J. L., Moss, S. A., Brereton, A. V., & Tonge, B. J. (2000). Atypical interference of local detail on global processing in high-functioning autism and Asperger’s disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 41(6), 769–778.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Ring, H. A., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Williams, S. C., Brammer, M., Andrew, C., et al. (1999). Cerebral correlates of preserved cognitive skills in autism: A functional MRI study of embedded figures task performance. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 122(Pt 7), 1305–1315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Rolls, E. T., Cowey, A., & Bruce, V. (1992). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying face processing within and beyond the temporal cortical visual areas [and discussion]. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 335, 11–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Rondan, C., & Deruelle, C. (2007). Global and configural visual processing in adults with autism and Asperger syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 28(2), 197–206. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2006.02.007.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Rose, F. E., Lincoln, A. J., Lai, Z., Ene, M., Searcy, Y. M., & Bellugi, U. (2007). Orientation and affective expression effects on face recognition in Williams syndrome and autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(3), 513–522. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0200-4.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Rosset, D. B., Rondan, C., Da Fonseca, D., Santos, A., Assouline, B., & Deruelle, C. (2008). Typical emotion processing for cartoon but not for real faces in children with autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(5), 919–925. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0465-2.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Rossion, B. (2008). Picture-plane inversion leads to qualitative changes of face perception. Acta Psychologica, 128(2), 274–289. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.02.003.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Rutherford, M. D., Clements, K. A., & Sekuler, A. B. (2007). Differences in discrimination of eye and mouth displacement in autism spectrum disorders. Vision Research, 47(15), 2099–2110. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2007.01.029.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Rutter, M. (2003). Introduction: Autism—the challenges ahead. Novartis Foundation Symposium, 251, 1–9. discussion 109–111, 281–197.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Sangrigoli, S., & de Schonen, S. (2004). Effect of visual experience on face processing: A developmental study of inversion and non-native effects. Developmental Science, 7(1), 74–87.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Sasson, N. J. (2006). The development of face processing in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(3), 381–394.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Scherf, K. S., Behrmann, M., & Dahl, R. E. (2012). Facing changes and changing faces in adolescence: A new model for investigating adolescent-specific interactions between pubertal, brain and behavioral development. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2(2), 199–219. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.07.016.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Scherf, K. S., Behrmann, M., Minshew, N., & Luna, B. (2008). Atypical development of face and greeble recognition in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 49(8), 838–847. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01903.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Schultz, R. T. (2005). Developmental deficits in social perception in autism: The role of the amygdala and fusiform face area. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23(2–3), 125–141. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2004.12.012.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Schultz, R. T., Gauthier, I., Klin, A., Fulbright, R. K., Anderson, A. W., Volkmar, F., et al. (2000). Abnormal ventral temporal cortical activity during face discrimination among individuals with autism and Asperger syndrome. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57(4), 331–340.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Sekuler, A. B., Gaspar, C. M., Gold, J. M., & Bennett, P. J. (2004). Inversion leads to quantitative, not qualitative, changes in face processing. Current Biology, 14(5), 391–396. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.02.028.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1983). An islet of ability in autistic children: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 24(4), 613–620.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Charman, T., Cox, A., Baird, G., Drew, A., et al. (1998). The frequency and distribution of spontaneous attention shifts between social and nonsocial stimuli in autistic, typically developing, and nonautistic developmentally delayed infants. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 39(5), 747–753.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Tanaka, J. W., & Corneille, O. (2007). Typicality effects in face and object perception: Further evidence for the attractor field model. Perception & Psychophysics, 69(4), 619–627.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Tanaka, J. W., & Farah, M. J. (1993). Parts and wholes in face recognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A-Human Experimental Psychology, 46(2), 225–245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Taylor, M. M., & Creelman, C. D. (1967). PEST: Efficiency estimates on probability functions. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 41, 782–787.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Teunisse, J. P., & de Gelder, B. (2003). Face processing in adolescents with autistic disorder: The inversion and composite effects. Brain and Cognition, 52(3), 285–294.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Valentine, T. (1988). Upside-down faces: A review of the effect of inversion upon face recognition. British Journal of Psychology, 79(Pt 4), 471–491.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. van der Geest, J. N., Kemner, C., Verbaten, M. N., & van Engeland, H. (2002). Gaze behavior of children with pervasive developmental disorder toward human faces: A fixation time study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 43(5), 669–678.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Vlamings, P. H., Jonkman, L. M., van Daalen, E., van der Gaag, R. J., & Kemner, C. (2010). Basic abnormalities in visual processing affect face processing at an early age in autism spectrum disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 68(12), 1107–1113. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.06.024.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Wechsler, D. (1939). The measurement of adult intelligence. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Witkins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Weigelt, S., Koldewyn, K., & Kanwisher, N. (2012). Face identity recognition in autism spectrum disorders: A review of behavioral studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(3), 1060–1084. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.12.008.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. Wichmann, F. A., & Hill, N. J. (2001). The psychometric function: I. Fitting, sampling and goodness-of-fit. Perception & Psychophysics, 63(8), 1293–1313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  95. Wilson, C. E., Brock, J., & Palermo, R. (2010a). Attention to social stimuli and facial identity recognition skills in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 54(12), 1104–1115. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2010.01340.x.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Wilson, C. E., Palermo, R., Brock, J., & Burton, A. M. (2010b). Learning new faces in typically developing children and children on the autistic spectrum. Perception, 39(12), 1645–1658.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. Wilson, H. R., Pascalis, O., & Blades, M. (2007). Familiar face recognition in children with autism; the differential use of inner and outer face parts. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(2), 314–320.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Wolf, J. M., Tanaka, J. W., Klaiman, C., Cockburn, J., Herlihy, L., Brown, C., et al. (2008). Specific impairment of face-processing abilities in children with autism spectrum disorder using the Let’s Face It! skills battery. Autism Research: Official Journal of the International Society for Autism Research, 1(6), 329–340. doi: 10.1002/aur.56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Yamashita, J. A., Hardy, J. L., De Valois, K. K., & Webster, M. A. (2005). Stimulus selectivity of figural aftereffects for faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 420–437.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  100. Young, A. W., Hellawell, D., & Hay, D. C. (1987). Configurational information in face perception. Perception, 16(6), 747–759.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  101. Zhao, L., & Chubb, C. (2001). The size-tuning of the face-distortion after-effect. Vision Research, 41, 2979–2994.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  102. Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., Rogers, T., Roberts, W., Brian, J., & Szatmari, P. (2005). Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of life. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23(2–3), 143–152. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2004.05.001.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  • Pamela M. Pallett
    • 1
    • 2
  • Shereen J. Cohen
    • 2
  • Karen R. Dobkins
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of Electrical and Computer EngineeringThe Ohio State UniversityColumbusUSA
  2. 2.Department of Psychology, 0109University of California, San DiegoLa JollaUSA

Personalised recommendations