Skip to main content
Log in

Profound Expressive Language Impairment in Low Functioning Children with Autism: An Investigation of Syntactic Awareness Using a Computerised Learning Task

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Nine low-functioning children with profound expressive language impairment and autism were studied in terms of their responsiveness to a computer-based learning program designed to assess syntactic awareness. The children learned to touch words on a screen in the correct sequence in order to see a corresponding animation, such as ‘monkey flies’. The game progressed in levels from 2 to 4 word sequences, contingent upon success at each stage. Although performance was highly variable across participants, a detailed review of their learning profiles suggested that no child lacked syntactic awareness and that elementary syntactic control in a non-speech domain was superior to that manifest in their spoken language. The reasons for production failures at the level of speech in children with autism are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. It is recognised that this term can sometimes refer to severe symptomatology rather than IQ.

  2. Programming carried out by Karl Monsen.

  3. Macromedia was recently acquired by Adobe. However, version 5 was released under the name Macromedia.

  4. Note that, for this reason, the lines plotted don’t always appear to denote a terminal performance at each level of 8/10.

  5. Prior to this, differences in RT sequences between similar levels were compared for each participant individually. No participant showed consistent RT differences between pairs of similar levels, supporting the combination of data across levels.

References

  • Beglinger, L. J., & Smith, T. H. (2001). A review of subtyping in autism and proposed dimensional classification model. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(4), 411–422.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beukeleman, D. R., & Mirenda, P. (1999). Augmentative and alternative communication: Management of severe communication disorders in children and adults (2nd ed.). Baltimore: M.D. Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boddaert, N., Chabane, N., Belin, P., Bourgeois, M., Royer, V., Barthelemy, C., et al. (2004). Perception of complex sounds in autism: Abnormal auditory cortical processing in children. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161(11), 2117–2120.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bonvilian, J. D., & Nelson, K. E. (1976). Sign language acquisition in a mute autistic boy. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 41, 339–347.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boucher, J. (2012). Structural language in autistic spectrum disorder: Characteristics and causes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 219–233.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bryson, S. E., Clark, B. S., & Smith, I. M. (1988). First report of a Canadian epidemiological study of autistic syndromes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 29(4), 433–445.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Casanova, M. F. (2006). Neurological and pathological findings in autism: The significance of a putative minicolumnopathy. The Neuroscientist, 12(5), 435–441.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Charlop-Christy, M. H., Carpenter, M., Le Blanc, L. A., & Kellet, K. (2002). Using the picture exchange communication system (PECS). Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 213–231.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cox, R. D., & Mesibov, G. B. (1995). Relationship between autism and learning disabilities. In E. Schopler & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.), Learning and cognition in autism (pp. 57–70). New York: Plenum.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • De Fosse, L., Hodge, S. M., Makris, N., Kennedy, D. N., Caviness, V. S., McGrath, L., et al. (2004). Language-association cortex asymmetry in autism and specific language impairment. Annals of Neurology, 56, 757–766.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dinstein, I., Pierce, K., Eyler, L., Solso, S., Malach, R., Behrmann, M., et al. (2011). Disrupted neural synchronization in toddlers with autism. Neuron, 70(6), 1218–1225.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fombonne, E. (2003). Epidemiological surveys of autism and other pervasive developmental disorders: An update. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(4), 365–382.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fulwiler, R. L., & Fouts, R. S. (1976). Acquisition of American sign language by a non-communicating autistic child. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 6, 43–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1993). Working memory and language. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gernsbacher, M. A., Sauer, E. A., Geye, H. M., Schweigert, E. K., & Goldsmith, H. H. (2008). Infant and toddler oral- and manual-motor skills predict later speech fluency in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(1), 43–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gleason, J. B. (2005). The development of language. Boston, MA: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, H. (2002). Communication intervention for children with autism: A review of treatment efficacy. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(5), 373–396.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, K., Pasco, G., McElduff, F., Wade, A., Howlin, P., & Charman, T. (2011). A communication-based intervention for nonverbal children with autism: What changes? Who benefits? Journal of Consulting Clinical Psychology, 79(4), 447–457.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Howlin, P. (1981). The effectiveness of operant language training with children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 11(1), 89–105.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Howlin, P. (2006). Augmentative and alternative communication systems for children with autism. In T. Charman & W. Stone (Eds.), Social and communication development in autism spectrum disorders (pp. 236–266). London: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joshi, A. K. (1987). An introduction to tree adjoining grammars. In A. Manaster-Ramer (Ed.), Mathematics of language (pp. 87–114). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Just, M. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Keller, T. A., & Minshew, N. J. (2004). Cortical activation and synchronization during sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism: Evidence of underconnectivity. Brain, 127, 1811–1821.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kjelgaard, M. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). An investigation of language impairment in autism: Implications for genetic sub-groups. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16(2/3), 287–308.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Locke, J. L. (1993). The child’s path to spoken language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Jr, Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 659–685.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Shulman, C., & Di Lavore, P. (2004). Regression and word loss in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(5), 936–955.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maljaars, J., Noens, I., Jansen, R., Scholte, E., & van Berckelaer-Onnes, I. (2011). Intentional communication in nonverbal and verbal low-functioning children with autism. Journal of Communication Disorders, 44(6), 601–604.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGonigle, B., & Chalmers, M. (2002). A behavior-based fractionation of cognitive competence with clinical applications: A comparative approach. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 15, 154–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mirenda, P. I., & Ianocu, T. (2008). Autism spectrum disorders and AAC. Baltimore: Brookes Publishing Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Hearn, K. C., Asato, M., Ordaz, S., & Luna, B. (2008). Neurodevelopment and executive function in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 20(4), 1103–1132.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1999). Words and rules: The ingredients of language.

  • Pinker, S., & Jackendoff, R. (2005). The faculty of language: What’s special about it? Cognition, 95, 201–236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. (1997). Autism as an executive disorder. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russo, N., Foxe, J. J., Brandwein, A. B., Altschuler, T., Gomes, H., & Molholm, S. (2010). Multisensory processing in children with autism: High-density electrical mapping of auditory-somatosensory integration. Autism Research, 3, 1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russo, N., Nicol, T., Trommer, B., Zecker, S., & Kraus, N. (2009). Brainstem transcription of speech is disrupted in children with autism spectrum disorders. Developmental Science, 12(4), 557–567.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Savage-Rumbaugh, S. (1986). Ape language: From conditioned response to symbol. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H., Paul, R., & Lord, C. (2005). Language and communication in autism. In F. R. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Autism and pervasive developmental disorders (Vol. 1, pp. 335–364). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Terrace, H. S., & McGonigle, B. (1994). Memory and representation of serial order by children, monkeys, and pigeons. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3(6), 180–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tiegerman, E. (1993). Autism spectrum disorders: Learning to communicate. In D. Bernstein & E. Tiegerman (Eds.), Language and communication disorders in children (3rd ed., pp. 314–369). Columbus, OH: Merrill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walker, M. (1980). Makaton vocabulary (rev ed.). Surrey, UK: The Makaton Vocabulary Development Project.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitehouse, A. J. O., Barry, J. G., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2008). Further defining the language impairment of autism: Is there a specific language impairment subtype? Journal of Communication Disorders, 41(4), 319–336.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • WHO. (1992). ICD-10 classifications of mental and behavioural disorder: Clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines.

  • Wickelgren, I. (2005). Autistic brains out of synch? Science, 308, 1856–1858.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yirmiya, N., & Charman, T. (2010). The prodrome of autism: early behavioral and biological signs, regression, peri- and post-natal development and genetics. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(4), 432–458.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. D., Burack, J. A., Boseovski, J. J., Jacques, S., & Frye, D. (2001). A cognitive complexity and control framework for the study of autism. In J. A. Burack, T. Charman, N. Yirmiya, & P. D. Zelazo (Eds.), The development of autism: Perspectives from theory and research (pp. 195–218). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The first author would like to gratefully acknowledge support from the British Academy through small project grants R39501, J29417 enabling construction and development of the core Eventaurs programme by Graham Sortino (programmer) and Stephen Elphick (animations), together with grant RA0558 that enabled the project described here. She would also like to thank Professor Uta Frith for her support and enthusiasm for the project at its inception. Finally, many thanks are due to the patience and co-operation of the children who participated in the project and that of their parents and families.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Maggie McGonigle-Chalmers.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

McGonigle-Chalmers, M., Alderson-Day, B., Fleming, J. et al. Profound Expressive Language Impairment in Low Functioning Children with Autism: An Investigation of Syntactic Awareness Using a Computerised Learning Task. J Autism Dev Disord 43, 2062–2081 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1753-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1753-z

Keywords

Navigation